Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition
- PMID: 10097009
- DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x97001611
Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition
Abstract
To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level--more abstract than that used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as reasoning. At this "embodiment level," the constraints of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations. The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural computational economies of sequential decision systems through a system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs. This target article focuses on how deictic binding make it possible to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism for representing the essential features that link external sensory data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye movements and hand movements.
Similar articles
-
Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements.Brain Cogn. 2008 Dec;68(3):327-40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.021. Brain Cogn. 2008. PMID: 19028265 Review.
-
Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space.J Neurophysiol. 1997 Sep;78(3):1601-18. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1601. J Neurophysiol. 1997. PMID: 9310446 Clinical Trial.
-
Balancing energetic and cognitive resources: memory use during search depends on the orienting effector.Cognition. 2014 Sep;132(3):443-54. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.005. Epub 2014 Jun 16. Cognition. 2014. PMID: 24946208 Clinical Trial.
-
Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention.Nat Neurosci. 2003 Feb;6(2):182-9. doi: 10.1038/nn996. Nat Neurosci. 2003. PMID: 12514738
-
Taking a new look at looking at nothing.Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Nov;12(11):405-10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.007. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008. PMID: 18805041 Review.
Cited by
-
Consequences of cognitive offloading: Boosting performance but diminishing memory.Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2021 Sep;74(9):1477-1496. doi: 10.1177/17470218211008060. Epub 2021 Apr 4. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2021. PMID: 33752519 Free PMC article.
-
The effect of metacognitive training on confidence and strategic reminder setting.PLoS One. 2020 Oct 23;15(10):e0240858. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240858. eCollection 2020. PLoS One. 2020. PMID: 33095817 Free PMC article.
-
Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand.J Exp Psychol Gen. 2010 Nov;139(4):665-82. doi: 10.1037/a0020198. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2010. PMID: 20853993 Free PMC article.
-
Task-dependence in scene perception: Head unrestrained viewing using mobile eye-tracking.J Vis. 2020 May 11;20(5):3. doi: 10.1167/jov.20.5.3. J Vis. 2020. PMID: 32392286 Free PMC article.
-
Perseveration on cognitive strategies.Mem Cognit. 2024 Apr;52(3):459-475. doi: 10.3758/s13421-023-01475-7. Epub 2023 Oct 24. Mem Cognit. 2024. PMID: 37874485 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Other Literature Sources