Motion discrimination in cortically blind patients
- PMID: 11133785
- DOI: 10.1093/brain/124.1.30
Motion discrimination in cortically blind patients
Abstract
Some patients with brain damage affecting the striate cortex, though clinically blind in their field defects, can still discriminate visual stimuli when forced choice procedures are used. Such patients seem particularly sensitive to moving stimuli in their scotomata, though there are conflicting reports as to whether they can discriminate the direction of motion. We tested three patients with areas of cortical blindness for their ability to detect and discriminate the direction of motion of a variety of first-order motion stimuli, namely bars, gratings, plaids and random dot kinematograms depicting translation and motion in depth, during forced choice tasks. The patients could detect the presence of movement in any kind of stimulus, and could discriminate the direction of single bars, but none could discriminate the direction of motion of the more complex stimuli (gratings, plaids and random dot kinematograms) or discriminate between 0 and 100% coherent random dot kinematograms at any speed tested (from 4 to 64 degrees /s). Similar results were obtained from one of the patients who was additionally tested with second-order versions of the translated bar and random dot kinematograms, eliminating light scatter as an explanation. Overall, the results suggest that motion processing in the scotoma is severely impaired, and that the puzzling discrepancies between previous studies can be accounted for by the type of stimulus used. The motion discrimination impairment caused by brain damage affecting the primary visual cortex is inconsistent with the proposed existence of a subcortical pathway to extrastriate cortical motion areas (such as areas MT and MST) which bypasses the striate cortex and is specialized for analysing 'fast' motion.
Similar articles
-
Pattern electroretinograms after cerebral hemispherectomy.Brain. 2001 Jun;124(Pt 6):1228-40. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.6.1228. Brain. 2001. PMID: 11353738
-
Direction discrimination of moving gratings and plaids and coherence in dot displays without primary visual cortex (V1).Eur J Neurosci. 1998 Dec;10(12):3767-72. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00383.x. Eur J Neurosci. 1998. PMID: 9875355
-
A lesion of cortical area V2 selectively impairs the perception of the direction of first-order visual motion.Neuroreport. 2000 Apr 7;11(5):1039-44. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004070-00028. Neuroreport. 2000. PMID: 10790879
-
The motion vision of the blind and the modularity of consciousness.Trans Med Soc Lond. 1995-1996;112:11-8. Trans Med Soc Lond. 1995. PMID: 9604080 Review.
-
Measurement of dynamic stereoacuity and global stereopsis.Aust J Ophthalmol. 1980 Feb;8(1):35-46. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9071.1980.tb00886.x. Aust J Ophthalmol. 1980. PMID: 6992757 Review.
Cited by
-
The cortical basis of global motion detection in blindsight.Exp Brain Res. 2009 Jan;192(3):407-11. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1508-4. Epub 2008 Jul 30. Exp Brain Res. 2009. PMID: 18665355
-
Role of inter-hemispheric transfer in generating visual evoked potentials in V1-damaged brain hemispheres.Neuropsychologia. 2015 Feb;68:82-93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.003. Epub 2015 Jan 7. Neuropsychologia. 2015. PMID: 25575450 Free PMC article.
-
Visual cortical activity reflects faster accumulation of information from cortically blind fields.Brain. 2012 Nov;135(Pt 11):3440-52. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws272. Brain. 2012. PMID: 23169923 Free PMC article.
-
Chromatic priming in hemianopic visual fields.Exp Brain Res. 2003 Sep;152(1):95-105. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1521-6. Epub 2003 Jul 23. Exp Brain Res. 2003. PMID: 12879180
-
Illusory motion perception in blindsight.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Jan 11;108(2):876-81. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005974108. Epub 2010 Dec 27. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011. PMID: 21187390 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources