Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass
- PMID: 11561921
- DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass
Abstract
Participants searched for discrepant fear-relevant pictures (snakes or spiders) in grid-pattern arrays of fear-irrelevant pictures belonging to the same category (flowers or mushrooms) and vice versa. Fear-relevant pictures were found more quickly than fear-irrelevant ones. Fear-relevant, but not fear-irrelevant, search was unaffected by the location of the target in the display and by the number of distractors, which suggests parallel search for fear-relevant targets and serial search for fear-irrelevant targets. Participants specifically fearful of snakes but not spiders (or vice versa) showed facilitated search for the feared objects but did not differ from controls in search for nonfeared fear-relevant or fear-irrelevant, targets. Thus, evolutionary relevant threatening stimuli were effective in capturing attention, and this effect was further facilitated if the stimulus was emotionally provocative.
Similar articles
-
Preparedness for action: responding to the snake in the grass.Am J Psychol. 2006 Spring;119(1):29-43. Am J Psychol. 2006. PMID: 16550854
-
Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: Evidence from attention and emotion measures.Behav Res Ther. 2009 Dec;47(12):1032-42. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022. Epub 2009 Aug 6. Behav Res Ther. 2009. PMID: 19695561
-
Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) quickly detect snakes but not spiders: Evolutionary origins of fear-relevant animals.J Comp Psychol. 2016 Aug;130(3):299-303. doi: 10.1037/com0000032. Epub 2016 Apr 14. J Comp Psychol. 2016. PMID: 27078076
-
Fear in infancy: Lessons from snakes, spiders, heights, and strangers.Dev Psychol. 2019 Sep;55(9):1889-1907. doi: 10.1037/dev0000675. Dev Psychol. 2019. PMID: 31464493 Free PMC article. Review.
-
The role of the amygdala in human fear: automatic detection of threat.Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005 Nov;30(10):953-8. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.019. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005. PMID: 15963650 Review.
Cited by
-
Information content moderates positivity and negativity biases in memory.Psychol Aging. 2013 Sep;28(3):853-63. doi: 10.1037/a0031440. Epub 2013 Feb 18. Psychol Aging. 2013. PMID: 23421322 Free PMC article. Clinical Trial.
-
Emotions' impact on viewing behavior under natural conditions.PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e52737. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052737. Epub 2013 Jan 9. PLoS One. 2013. PMID: 23326353 Free PMC article. Clinical Trial.
-
Metamemory and Memory Discrepancies in Directed Forgetting of Emotional Information.Eur J Psychol. 2021 Feb 26;17(1):44-52. doi: 10.5964/ejop.2567. eCollection 2021 Feb. Eur J Psychol. 2021. PMID: 33737973 Free PMC article.
-
Neural correlates of the behavioral-autonomic interaction response to potentially threatening stimuli.Front Hum Neurosci. 2013 Jan 16;6:349. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00349. eCollection 2012. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013. PMID: 23335893 Free PMC article.
-
Brief report: Effect of spatial complexity on visual short-term memory and self-reported autistic-like traits in typically developed individuals.J Autism Dev Disord. 2013 Jul;43(7):1725-31. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1711-9. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013. PMID: 23132273
References
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources