Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface
- PMID: 12485740
- DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00179-8
Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface
Abstract
In this article we present two sets of experiments designed to investigate the acquisition of scalar implicatures. Scalar implicatures arise in examples like Some professors are famous where the speaker's use of some typically indicates that s/he had reasons not to use a more informative term, e.g. all. Some professors are famous therefore gives rise to the implicature that not all professors are famous. Recent studies on the development of pragmatics suggest that preschool children are often insensitive to such implicatures when they interpret scalar terms (Cognition 78 (2001) 165; Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Guasti, M.T., Gualmini, A., & Meroni, L. (2001). The acquisition of disjunction: evidence for a grammatical view of scalar implicatures. In A.H.-J. Do, L. Dominguez, & A. Johansen (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 157-168). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press; Musolino, J., & Lidz, J. (2002). Preschool logic: truth and felicity in the acquisition of quantification. In B. Skarabela, S. Fish, & A.H.-J. Do, Proceedings of the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 406-416). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press). This conclusion raises two important questions: (a) are all scalar terms treated in the same way by young children?, and (b) does the child's difficulty reflect a genuine inability to derive scalar implicatures or is it due to demands imposed by the experimental task on an otherwise pragmatically savvy child? Experiment 1 addresses the first question by testing a group of 30 5-year-olds and 30 adults (all native speakers of Greek) on three different scales, <oli, meriki> (<all, some>), <tris, dio> (<three, two>) and <teliono, arxizo> (<finish, start>). In each case, subjects were presented with contexts which satisfied the semantic content of the stronger (i.e. more informative) terms on each scale (i.e. all, three and finish) but were described using the weaker terms of the scales (i.e. some, two, start). We found that, while adults overwhelmingly rejected these infelicitous descriptions, children almost never did so. Children also differed from adults in that their rejection rate on the numerical scale was reliably higher than on the two other scales. In order to address question (b), we trained a group of 30 5-year-olds to detect infelicitous statements. We then presented them with modified versions of the stories of Experiment 1, which now more readily invited scalar inferences. These manipulations gave rise to significantly higher rejection rates than those observed in Experiment 1. Overall, these findings indicate that children do not treat all scalar terms alike and, more importantly, that children's ability to derive scalar implicatures is affected by their awareness of the goal of the task. Developmental and methodological implications as well as theoretical implications for the semantics of numeral terms are discussed.
Copyright 2002 Elsevier Science B.V.
Similar articles
-
The logical syntax of number words: theory, acquisition and processing.Cognition. 2009 Apr;111(1):24-45. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.12.008. Epub 2009 Feb 13. Cognition. 2009. PMID: 19217616
-
On children's variable success with scalar inferences: Insights from disjunction in the scope of a universal quantifier.Cognition. 2018 Sep;178:178-192. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.020. Epub 2018 Jun 4. Cognition. 2018. PMID: 29879554
-
From scalar semantics to implicature: children's interpretation of aspectuals.J Child Lang. 2006 Nov;33(4):721-57. doi: 10.1017/s0305000906007550. J Child Lang. 2006. PMID: 17153860
-
Pragmatic responses to under-informative some-statements are not scalar implicatures.Cognition. 2023 Aug;237:105463. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105463. Epub 2023 Apr 13. Cognition. 2023. PMID: 37060849 Review.
-
Priming scalar and ad hoc enrichment in children.Cognition. 2023 Oct;239:105572. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105572. Epub 2023 Jul 24. Cognition. 2023. PMID: 37494789 Review.
Cited by
-
Context in Generalized Conversational Implicatures: The Case of Some.Front Psychol. 2016 Mar 22;7:381. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00381. eCollection 2016. Front Psychol. 2016. PMID: 27047413 Free PMC article.
-
Conversational pragmatics: memory reporting strategies in different social contexts.Front Psychol. 2023 May 25;14:1004524. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1004524. eCollection 2023. Front Psychol. 2023. PMID: 37303885 Free PMC article.
-
On the role of entailment patterns and scalar implicatures in the processing of numerals.J Mem Lang. 2009 Nov 1;61(4):503-518. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.07.005. J Mem Lang. 2009. PMID: 20161494 Free PMC article.
-
The comprehension and production of quantifiers in isiXhosa-speaking Grade 1 learners.S Afr J Commun Disord. 2016 May 20;63(2):e1-e10. doi: 10.4102/sajcd.v63i2.138. S Afr J Commun Disord. 2016. PMID: 27245132 Free PMC article.
-
The development of optimistic expectations in young children.Cogn Dev. 2022 Jul-Sep;63:101201. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101201. Epub 2022 Jun 9. Cogn Dev. 2022. PMID: 39131667 Free PMC article.
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Miscellaneous