Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey
- PMID: 12711711
- DOI: 10.1152/jn.00656.2002
Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey
Abstract
Saccades that consistently over- or undershoot their targets gradually become smaller or larger, respectively. The signal that elicits adaptation of saccade size is a difference between eye and target positions appearing repeatedly at the ends of saccades. Here we describe how visual error size affects the size of saccade adaptation. At the end of each saccade, we imposed a constant-sized error by moving the target to a specified point relative to eye position. We tested a variety of error sizes imposed after saccades to target movements of 6, 12, and 18 degrees. We found that the size of the gain change elicited in a particular experiment depended on both the size of the imposed postsaccade error and on the size of the preceding target movement. For example, imposed errors of 4-5 degrees reduce saccades tracking 6, 12, and 18 degrees target movements by an average of 18, 35, and 45%, respectively. The most effective errors were those that were 15-45% of the size of the initial target eccentricity. Negative errors, which reduce saccade size, were more effective in changing saccade gain than were positive errors, which increased saccade size. For example, for 12 degrees target movements, negative and positive errors of 2-6 degrees changed saccade gain an average of 35 and 8%, respectively. This description of the relationship between error size and adaptation size improves our ability to adapt saccades in the laboratory and characterizes the error sizes that will best drive neurons carrying the adaptation-related visual error signal.
Similar articles
-
Distinct short-term and long-term adaptation to reduce saccade size in monkey.J Neurophysiol. 2006 Sep;96(3):1030-41. doi: 10.1152/jn.01151.2005. Epub 2006 May 3. J Neurophysiol. 2006. PMID: 16672299
-
The influence of the consistency of postsaccadic visual errors on saccadic adaptation.J Neurophysiol. 2010 Jun;103(6):3302-10. doi: 10.1152/jn.00970.2009. Epub 2010 Apr 14. J Neurophysiol. 2010. PMID: 20393067
-
Frontal eye field contributions to rapid corrective saccades.J Neurophysiol. 2007 Feb;97(2):1457-69. doi: 10.1152/jn.00433.2006. Epub 2006 Nov 29. J Neurophysiol. 2007. PMID: 17135479
-
Adaptive modification of saccade size produces correlated changes in the discharges of fastigial nucleus neurons.J Neurophysiol. 2003 Aug;90(2):1011-26. doi: 10.1152/jn.00193.2002. J Neurophysiol. 2003. PMID: 12904501
-
The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity.Prog Neurobiol. 2004 Jan;72(1):27-53. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2003.12.002. Prog Neurobiol. 2004. PMID: 15019175 Review.
Cited by
-
End-point variability is not noise in saccade adaptation.PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059731. Epub 2013 Mar 21. PLoS One. 2013. PMID: 23555763 Free PMC article.
-
Short-term adaptation of the cervico-ocular reflex.Exp Brain Res. 2004 May;156(1):124-8. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1878-1. Epub 2004 Mar 10. Exp Brain Res. 2004. PMID: 15014925
-
Interactions between sensory prediction error and task error during implicit motor learning.PLoS Comput Biol. 2022 Mar 23;18(3):e1010005. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010005. eCollection 2022 Mar. PLoS Comput Biol. 2022. PMID: 35320276 Free PMC article.
-
No evidence for differential saccadic adaptation in children and adults with an autism spectrum diagnosis.Front Integr Neurosci. 2023 Oct 6;17:1232474. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2023.1232474. eCollection 2023. Front Integr Neurosci. 2023. PMID: 37869448 Free PMC article.
-
Selective reward affects the rate of saccade adaptation.Neuroscience. 2017 Jul 4;355:113-125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.048. Epub 2017 May 10. Neuroscience. 2017. PMID: 28499971 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources