Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2003 Jun;85(3):441-50.
doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00064-6.

Effect of typicality on online category verification of animate category exemplars in aphasia

Affiliations

Effect of typicality on online category verification of animate category exemplars in aphasia

Swathi Kiran et al. Brain Lang. 2003 Jun.

Abstract

Normal young, elderly, Broca's aphasic, and Wernicke's aphasic individuals participated in an online category verification task where primes were superordinate category labels while targets were either typical or atypical examples of animate categories or nonmembers belonging to inanimate categories. The reaction time to judge whether the target belonged to the preceding category label was measured. Results indicated that all four groups made significantly greater errors on atypical examples compared to typical examples. Young and elderly individuals, and Broca's aphasic patients performed similarly on the verification task; these groups demonstrated faster reaction times on typical examples than atypical examples. Wernicke's aphasic patients made the most errors on the task and were slowest to respond than any other participant group. Also, these participants were not significantly faster at accepting correct typical examples compared to correct atypical examples. The results from the four groups are discussed with relevance to prototype/family resemblance models of typicality.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Error percentage for typical, atypical examples, and nonmembers across the four participant groups. Errors for each category are collapsed within typicality.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Mean RTs and error bars for typical, atypical examples, and nonmembers across the four participant groups. RTs for categories are collapsed within typicality.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Advantage for the typical example over the atypical example (in percentage) for the four participant groups.

References

    1. Bruner JS, Goodnow J, Austin G. A study of thinking. New York: Wiley; 1956.
    1. Caramazza A, Shelton J. Domain specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate–inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1998;10:1–34. - PubMed
    1. Cardebrat D, Demonet JF, Celsis P, Fuel M. Living/non-living dissociation in a case of semantic dementia: A SPECT activation study. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:1175–1179. - PubMed
    1. Casey PJ. A re-examination of the roles of typicality and category dominance in verifying category membership. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1992;18(4):823–834. - PubMed
    1. Frances N, Kucera H. Frequency analysis of English usage. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1982.