Variation among developmental dyslexics: evidence from a printed-word-learning task
- PMID: 14757068
- DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.004
Variation among developmental dyslexics: evidence from a printed-word-learning task
Abstract
A word-learning task was used to investigate variation among developmental dyslexics classified as phonological and surface dyslexics. Dyslexic children and chronological age (CA)- and reading level (RL)-matched normal readers were taught to pronounce novel nonsense words such as veep. Words were assigned either a regular (e.g., "veep") or an irregular (e.g., "vip") pronunciation. Phonological dyslexics learned both regular and exception words more slowly than the normal readers and, unlike the other groups, did not show a regular-word advantage. Surface dyslexics also learned regular and exception words more slowly than the CA group, consistent with a specific problem in mastering arbitrary item-specific pronunciations, but their performance resembled that of the RL group. The results parallel earlier findings from Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, McBride-Chang, & Petersen [Cognition 58 (1996) 157-195] indicating that surface dyslexics and phonological dyslexics have a different profile of reading deficits, with surface dyslexics resembling younger normal readers and phonological dyslexics showing a specific phonological deficit. Models of reading and reading disability need to account for the heterogeneity in reading processes among dyslexic children.
Similar articles
-
New approaches to the developmental dyslexias.Adv Neurol. 1984;42:223-32. Adv Neurol. 1984. PMID: 6507179
-
On the bases of two subtypes of developmental [corrected] dyslexia.Cognition. 1996 Feb;58(2):157-95. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6. Cognition. 1996. PMID: 8820386
-
Process dissociation of sight vocabulary and phonetic decoding in reading: a new perspective on surface and phonological dyslexias.Brain Lang. 2005 Feb;92(2):185-203. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.003. Brain Lang. 2005. PMID: 15629491
-
The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia.Dyslexia. 2001 Jan-Mar;7(1):12-36. doi: 10.1002/dys.186. Dyslexia. 2001. PMID: 11305228 Review.
-
Phonological dyslexia and phonological impairment: an exception to the rule?Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(14):2861-73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.006. Epub 2006 Aug 1. Neuropsychologia. 2006. PMID: 16879843 Review.
Cited by
-
The stability of literacy-related cognitive contributions to Chinese character naming and reading fluency.J Psycholinguist Res. 2013 Oct;42(5):433-50. doi: 10.1007/s10936-012-9228-0. J Psycholinguist Res. 2013. PMID: 22923217
-
Tracking orthographic learning in children with different profiles of reading difficulty.Front Hum Neurosci. 2014 Jul 4;8:468. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00468. eCollection 2014. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014. PMID: 25071504 Free PMC article.
-
Dyslexic adults can learn from repeated stimulus presentation but have difficulties in excluding external noise.PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e27893. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027893. Epub 2011 Nov 23. PLoS One. 2011. PMID: 22132164 Free PMC article.
-
Differential effects of orthographic and phonological consistency in cortex for children with and without reading impairment.Neuropsychologia. 2008 Dec;46(14):3210-24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.024. Neuropsychologia. 2008. PMID: 18725239 Free PMC article.
-
Anatomy is strategy: skilled reading differences associated with structural connectivity differences in the reading network.Brain Lang. 2014 Jun;133:1-13. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.005. Epub 2014 Apr 16. Brain Lang. 2014. PMID: 24735993 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources