Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2004 Jul 27;101(30):10862-7.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403402101. Epub 2004 Jul 19.

Transparallel processing by hyperstrings

Affiliations

Transparallel processing by hyperstrings

Peter A van der Helm. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

Human vision research aims at understanding the brain processes that enable us to see the world as a structured whole consisting of separate objects. To explain how humans organize a visual pattern, structural information theory starts from the idea that our visual system prefers the organization with the simplest descriptive code, that is, the code that captures a maximum of visual regularity. Empirically, structural information theory gained support from psychological data on a wide variety of perceptual phenomena, but theoretically, the computation of guaranteed simplest codes remained a troubling problem. Here, the graph-theoretical concept of "hyperstrings" is presented as a key to the solution of this problem. A hyperstring is a distributed data structure that allows a search for regularity in O(2(N)) strings as if only one string of length N were concerned. Thereby, hyperstrings enable transparallel processing, a previously uncharacterized form of processing that might also be a form of cognitive processing.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Suppose for the string 𝒫 = ababfabab that the regularity search has yielded simplest covering ISA-forms for the substrings of 𝒫 (only a few of these ISA-forms are shown). Then, the SPM yields S[(2*(ab)), (f)] as the simplest code of 𝒫. (Note: the number of string symbols in a code is taken to quantify its structural complexity.)
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
A hyperstring. The paths from vertex 1 to vertex 4 represent the same substrings as those represented by the paths from vertex 5 to vertex 8; that is, the substring sets π(1, 4) and π(5, 8) are identical.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
A hyperstring that represents 13 chunkings of the string abcfabcg. Just as in Fig. 2, the substring sets π(1, 4) and π(5, 8) are identical.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
The A-graph formula image for the string T = akagakakag, with three independent hyperstrings and with, among others, identical substring sets π(1, 5) and π(7, 11).
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
The S-graph formula image for the string T = ababfdedgpfdedgbaba, with two independent subgraphs. Dashed edges and bold edges represent pivots and S-chunks, respectively, in S-forms covering diafixes of T.
Fig. 6.
Fig. 6.
(a) The S-graph for the string T1 = ababfababgbabafbaba, with, among others, identical substring sets π(1, 5) and π(6, 10). (b) The S-graph for the nearly identical string T2 = ababfababgbabafabab, in which the substring sets π(1, 5) and π(6, 10) are disjunct.
Fig. 7.
Fig. 7.
For the hyperstring in the S-graph formula image from Fig. 6a, with T1 = ababfababgbabafbaba, the hierarchically recursive regularity search yields simplest covering ISA-forms for the hypersubstrings (only a few of these ISA-forms are shown). By including the pivots, the all-pairs SPM then yields simplest covering S-forms for the diafixes of T1 (only a few of these S-forms are shown). Note that the number of string symbols in a code is taken to quantify its structural complexity, and for clarity the substrings aba and bab inside chunks are shown uncoded but should be read as S[(a), (b)] and S[(b), (a)], respectively.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1968) Structural Information of Visual Patterns: An Efficient Coding System in Perception (Mouton, The Hague, The Netherlands).
    1. Hochberg, J. E. & McAlister, E. (1953) J. Exp. Psychol. 46, 361–364. - PubMed
    1. Shannon, C. E. (1948) Bell System Tech. J. 27, 379–423, 623–656.
    1. Koffka, K. (1935) Principles of Gestalt Psychology (Routledge & Kegan Paul, London).
    1. van der Helm, P. A., van Lier, R. & Wagemans, J., eds. (2003) Visual Gestalt Formation, special issue of Acta Psychol. 114, 211–398.

LinkOut - more resources