Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2005 Feb 3;45(3):469-77.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.039.

Transient attention enhances perceptual performance and FMRI response in human visual cortex

Affiliations

Transient attention enhances perceptual performance and FMRI response in human visual cortex

Taosheng Liu et al. Neuron. .

Abstract

When a visual stimulus suddenly appears, it captures attention, producing a transient improvement of performance on basic visual tasks. We investigate the effect of transient attention on stimulus representations in early visual areas using rapid event-related fMRI. Participants discriminated the orientation of one of two gratings preceded or followed by a nonpredictive peripheral cue. Compared to control conditions, precueing the target location improved performance and produced a larger fMRI response in corresponding retinotopic areas. This enhancement progressively increased from striate to extrastriate areas. Control conditions indicated that the enhanced fMRI response was not due to sensory summation of cue and target signals. Thus, an uninformative precue increases both perceptual performance and the concomitant stimulus-evoked activity in early visual areas. These results provide evidence regarding the retinotopically specific neural correlate for the effects of transient attention on early vision.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Experimental Design
The sequence of events in the precue and postcue trials is illustrated. Note that the onset of the Gabor stimuli within a trial is identical for the precue and postcue trials; both are 100 ms after trial onset. For the purpose of illustration, the Gabor stimuli are shown at a contrast of 50%, and the tilted Gabor stimuli are oriented ±10°.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Representative Results from the Localizer Scans
The diagram on the left illustrates the locations of the cue and the Gabor stimulus, which were presented in alternating blocks. Shown on the right are results from the right hemisphere of one participant, viewed on inflated surface representation of the posterior occipital cortex. Light and dark gray depict gyral and sulcal surfaces, respectively. Brain activity associated with the cue and brain activity associated with the Gabor stimulus are shown in blue and green maps, respectively. Black lines indicate the borders of early visual areas defined by the retinotopic mapping procedure (solid line, vertical meridian; dashed line, horizontal meridian). These borders were derived by using a wedge stimulus encompassing 0.25°–8.25° eccentricity, and the Gabor stimulus covered 4°–8° eccentricity. The asterisk indicates the foveal confluence where borders between areas cannot be resolved. At this statistical threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons), the activation of the cue and the Gabor did not overlap in V1, V2, and V3. Activity started to overlap in V3a and hV4, as they contain a hemifield representation (Tootell et al., 1997; Wade et al., 2002).
Figure 3
Figure 3. Group-Averaged Data
(A) Behavioral results. Proportion correct (left) and reaction time (right) are shown for the four cue conditions (V-Pre, valid precue; I-Pre, invalid precue; V-Post, valid postcue; I-Post, invalid postcue). Error bars are 1 SEM. (B) Imaging results. Mean fMRI responses across participants for each cue condition and distracter are shown for each visual area. Response was obtained from the dorsal (V1, V2, V3, and V3a) and ventral (hV4) representations of the target (the green areas in Figure 2). The average standard error of all time points along a curve is shown as the error bar on the first time point.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Individual Data
Representative data from two individual participants—a trained psychophysical observer (FP) and a naive observer (KM). For each participant, the top row shows behavioral results, and the middle and bottom rows show imaging results for different visual areas. (For details, see the legend of Figure 3.)
Figure 5
Figure 5. Peak fMRI Response
Peak amplitude of the fMRI response for different trial types in all visual areas (legends identical to Figure 3). Error bars are 1 SEM.
Figure 6
Figure 6. Magnitude of Attentional Effect
Mean attention modulation index (AMI) across participants for different visual areas [AMI = (Peakvalid precue − Peakbaseline)/(Peakvalid precue + Peakbaseline), where Peakbaseline = average of the peak amplitude for the invalid precue, valid postcue, and invalid postcue conditions]. Error bars are 1 SEM.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Boynton GM, Demb JB, Glover GH, Heeger DJ. Neuronal basis of contrast discrimination. Vision Res. 1999;39:257–269. - PubMed
    1. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10:433–436. - PubMed
    1. Brefczynski JA, DeYoe EA. A physiological correlate of the ‘spotlight’ of visual attention. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:370–374. - PubMed
    1. Cameron EL, Tai JC, Carrasco M. Covert attention affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity. Vision Res. 2002;42:949–967. - PubMed
    1. Carrasco M, McElree B. Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:5363–5367. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms

LinkOut - more resources