Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2006 Jul;32(3):271-83.
doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.271.

A comparator view of Pavlovian and differential inhibition

Affiliations
Comparative Study

A comparator view of Pavlovian and differential inhibition

Gonzalo P Urcelay et al. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2006 Jul.

Abstract

In 3 experiments using rats as subjects, the authors varied trial spacing to investigate the conditions under which Pavlovian and differential inhibition are observed. Experiment 1 compared Pavlovian and differential inhibition with spaced training trials. Spaced trials resulted in only the Pavlovian inhibitor passing both summation and retardation tests. Conversely, Experiment 2 compared these 2 types of inhibition with massed training trials. This training resulted in only the differential inhibitor passing both tests for conditioned inhibition. Finally, in Experiment 3 all subjects experienced Pavlovian inhibition training with massed trials. Although this training by itself did not result in behavior indicative of inhibition, subjects that also experienced posttraining extinction of the training context did pass both tests for inhibition. Overall, these results are anticipated by the extended comparator hypothesis (Denniston, Savastano, & Miller, 2001) but are problematic for most contemporary associative learning theories.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
A schematic representation of the extended comparator hypothesis (Denniston et al., 2001) applied to Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Here, the training excitor A is depicted as the target cue’s first-order comparator stimulus and the training context serves as the target cue’s second-order comparator stimulus. As it applies to differential inhibition, the context replaces A as the first-order comparator stimulus, given the absence of an X–A association, and there is no significant second-order comparator stimulus. Rectangles are physical stimuli and responses, ovals are stimulus representations, and the diamond is the actual comparator process.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean times to complete 5 cumulative seconds of drinking upon presentation of the target CS in Experiment 1. Left: Results of the summation test. Right: Results of the retardation test. Note that subjects that served as controls for the summation test (Ret groups) were the target groups for the retardation test, and vice versa. The actual test stimuli are indicated within parentheses. See Table 1 for the treatments of the four groups.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean times to complete 5 cumulative seconds of drinking upon presentation of the target CS in Experiment 2. Left: Results of the summation test. Right: Results of the retardation test. Note that subjects that served as controls for the summation test (Ret groups) were the target groups for the retardation test, and vice versa. See Table 1 for the treatments of the four groups.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean times to complete 5 cumulative seconds of drinking upon presentation of the target CS in Experiment 3. Left: Results of the summation test. Right: Results of the retardation test. Note that subjects that served as controls for the summation test (Ret groups) were the target groups for the retardation test, and vice versa. All subjects received Pavlovian inhibition training. See Table 2 for treatments of the four groups.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Blaisdell AP, Bristol AS, Gunther LM, Miller RR. Overshadowing and latent inhibition counteract each other: Support for the comparator hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1998;24:335–351. - PubMed
    1. Chang RC, Blaisdell AP, Miller RR. Backward conditioning: Mediation by the context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavioral Processes. 2003;29:171–183. - PubMed
    1. Couvillon PA, Ablan CD, Bitterman ME. Exploratory studies of inhibitory conditioning in honeybees (Apis mellifera) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1999;25:103–112. - PubMed
    1. Couvillon PA, Ablan CD, Ferreira TP, Bitterman ME. The role of nonreinforcement in learning of honeybees. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 2001;54B:127–144. - PubMed
    1. Couvillon PA, Hsiung R, Cooke AM, Bitterman ME. The role of context in the inhibitory conditioning of honeybees. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 2005;58B:59–67. - PubMed

Publication types