Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2007 Jan 15;578(Pt 2):387-96.
doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.120121. Epub 2006 Sep 28.

Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability

Affiliations
Review

Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability

Paul M Bays et al. J Physiol. .

Abstract

Sensory and motor noise limits the precision with which we can sense the world and act upon it. Recent research has begun to reveal computational principles by which the central nervous system reduces the sensory uncertainty and movement variability arising from this internal noise. Here we review the role of optimal estimation and sensory filtering in extracting the sensory information required for motor planning, and the role of optimal control, motor adaptation and impedance control in the specification of the motor output signal.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Optimal integration in the estimation of hand position
A, ellipses illustrate the precision of visual (red) and proprioceptive (blue) estimates of hand position in the horizontal plane. The narrower an ellipse in a certain direction, the more precise the estimate. (Adapted with permission from Van Beers et al. 2002.) B, the uncertainty in the sensory input can be represented as a probability distribution of possible locations. In depth (top), the proprioceptive estimate of position (blue) has a lower variance than the visual estimate (red). When there is a discrepancy between visual and proprioceptive estimates, the optimal combined estimate is obtained by weighting each source by the inverse of its variance. This combined estimate (green) has lower variance than either input alone. In azimuth (bottom), the relative precision of vision and proprioception is reversed, with the result that the optimal estimate now relies more on vision than proprioception. C, an estimate of position obtained from visual input alone can be improved by combining it with knowledge of a prior distribution of probable positions. The extent to which this prior distribution (grey) influences the optimal position estimate depends on the variance in the visual input. When visual feedback is very precise (top), the prior distribution has little effect, but when the variance of the visual estimate is increased (bottom), the optimal estimate becomes biased towards the mean of the prior distribution.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Predictive filtering of self-generated sensation
A, schematic representation of a sensory cancellation mechanism. On the basis of efference copy, a forward model predicts the sensory feedback that will result from a planned action. Subtracting this prediction from the actual sensory input reveals an estimate of the sensory feedback due to external influences. B, performance of healthy subjects (blue) and schizophrenic patients (red) on a force-matching task. Subjects were instructed to reproduce a target force, applied to the left index finger by a torque motor, either directly by pressing with the right index finger (circles) or indirectly by controlling the torque motor output with a joystick (squares). Average matching force is shown as a function of target force. Error bars indicate ± 1 s.e.m. across subjects. Dashed line represents perfect performance. Adapted with permission from Shergill et al. (2005).
Figure 3
Figure 3. Cost functions for optimal control
A, optimal control models attempt to minimize a specified ‘cost’, such as the error in position at the end of a movement. Cost functions are shown that minimize different features of position error: the squared error (top), the absolute error (middle), or the number of misses (i.e. non-zero errors, bottom). B, one of the asymmetric probability distributions of position used in Kording & Wolpert (2004b). Vertical lines indicate the point in the distribution that should be aligned with the target in order to minimize each of the cost functions shown in A. C, the continuous black line indicates the cost function inferred from the results of Kording & Wolpert (2004b); the grey area represents ± 1 s.e.m. across subjects. The squared error is shown for comparison (red dashed line). Adapted with permission from Kording & Wolpert (2004b).
Figure 4
Figure 4. Motor adaptation and impedance control during reaching movements
A, typical hand trajectories for out-and-back movements to targets (red) on first exposure to a velocity-dependent force field (top) and after 20 movement blocks (sets of 8 targets) in the field (bottom). Arrows (green) indicate the direction of force during outward movement. The increase in movement accuracy with training results from a strategy of motor adaptation, in which appropriate forces are generated to counteract the predictable force field. B, hand trajectories on first exposure to a divergent force field (top left), and after 100 movements in the field (top right). In this unpredictable force field, increased movement accuracy reflects a strategy of impedance control. Stiffness ellipses (bottom) indicate restoring force to a step displacement of the hand in different directions, before and after training. The increase in stiffness is tuned to the direction of maximum instability, perpendicular to the direction of motion (black arrow). Adapted with permission from Burdet et al. (2001).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar functions. Math Biosci. 1971;10:25–61.
    1. Andersson G, Armstrong DM. Complex spikes in Purkinje cells in the lateral vermis (b zone) of the cat cerebellum during locomotion. J Physiol. 1987;385:107–134. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Angel RW, Malenka RC. Velocity-dependent suppression of cutaneous sensitivity during movement. Exp Neurol. 1982;77:266–274. - PubMed
    1. Bahill AT, Clark MR, Stark L. The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements. Math Biosci. 1975;24:191–204.
    1. Bays PM, Flanagan JR, Wolpert DM. Attenuation of self-generated tactile sensations is predictive, not postdictive. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e28. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources