Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2006 Oct 4;26(40):10120-8.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-06.2006.

Individual differences in amygdala activity predict response speed during working memory

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Individual differences in amygdala activity predict response speed during working memory

Alexandre Schaefer et al. J Neurosci. .

Abstract

The human amygdala has classically been viewed as a brain structure primarily related to emotions and dissociated from higher cognition. We report here findings suggesting that the human amygdala also has a role in supporting working memory (WM), a canonical higher cognitive function. In a first functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study (n = 53), individual differences in amygdala activity predicted behavioral performance in a 3-back WM task. Specifically, higher event-related amygdala amplitude predicted faster response time (RT; r = -0.64), with no loss of accuracy. This relationship was not contingent on mood state, task content, or personality variables. In a second fMRI study (n = 21), we replicated the key finding (r = -0.47) and further showed that the correlation between the amygdala and faster RT was specific to a high working memory load condition (3-back) compared with a low working memory load condition (1-back). These results support models of amygdala function that can account for its involvement not only in emotion but also higher cognition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Schematic diagram of an n-back task. Stimuli are displayed sequentially on a screen and subjects are instructed to decide through a button press whether the current stimulus is equal or different from the stimulus displayed n trials back. In this example, n = 3.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Statistical map of Pearson correlations between brain activity and task response time in experiment 1 (threshold, p < 0.05, cluster-level corrected) showing clusters of negative correlations in the left and right amygdala on coronal (a), transverse (b), and sagittal (c) views (peak voxel correlation: x = −20, y = −6, z = −21).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Scatterplot of the peak voxel correlation between the left amygdala and response time in experiment 1 (after controlling for outliers): x = −20, y = −6, z = −21; r = −0.60; p = 0.000001.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Correlations between anatomical ROI of the left amygdala and task response time by WM load condition for experiment 2 (after controlling for outliers). Correlations are significant for the 3-back condition (r = −0.47; p < 0.05) and for the 1-back condition (r = 0.49; p < 0.05).
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Averaged time course of the left amygdala ROI by WM load condition and performance group for experiment 2 after baseline correction. The effect of cognitive load is specific to the H-P group (see Results).
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
The x-axis represents a difference in amygdala activity (3-back minus 1-back) using an anatomical ROI of the left amygdala, and the y-axis represents median response time for experiment 2 (after controlling for outliers). Correlations are significant for the 3-back condition (r = −0.73; p < 0.001) and for the 1-back condition (r = −0.63; p < 0.005).
Figure 7.
Figure 7.
Averaged time course of the right amygdala ROI by WM load condition and performance group for experiment 2. The main effect of cognitive load is significant independently of performance group (see Results).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aalto S, Bruck A, Laine M, Nagren K, Rinne JO. Frontal and temporal dopamine release during working memory and attention tasks in healthy humans: a positron emission tomography study using the high-affinity dopamine D2 receptor ligand [11C]FLB 457. J Neurosci. 2005;25:2471–2477. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson JR. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP; 1983. The architecture of cognition.
    1. Barch DM. Pharmacological manipulation of human working memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;174:126–135. - PubMed
    1. Braver TS, Cohen JD. On the control of control: the role of dopamine in regulating prefrontal function and working memory. In: Monsell S, Driver J, editors. Attention and performance. XVIII. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 2000. pp. 713–738.
    1. Braver TS, Ruge H. Functional neuroimaging of executive functions. In: Cabeza R, Kingstone A, editors. Functional neuroimaging of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 2006. pp. 307–347.

Publication types