The automaticity of emotional Stroop: a meta-analysis
- PMID: 17112461
- DOI: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.008
The automaticity of emotional Stroop: a meta-analysis
Abstract
An automatic bias to threat is often invoked to account for colour-naming interference in emotional Stroop. Recent findings by McKenna and Sharma [(2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: The role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 382-392], however, cast doubt on the fast and non-conscious nature of emotional Stroop. Interference by threat words only occurred with colour naming in the trial subsequent to the threat trial (i.e., a "slow" effect), but not immediately (i.e., a "fast" effect, as would be predicted by the bias hypothesis). In a meta-analysis of 70 published emotional Stroop studies the largest effects occurred when presentation of threat words was blocked, suggesting a strong contribution by slow interference. We did not find evidence; moreover, for interference in suboptimal (less conscious) presentation conditions and the only significant effects were observed in optimal (fully conscious) conditions with high-anxious non-clinical participants and patients. The emotional Stroop effect seems to rely more on a slow disengagement process than on a fast, automatic, bias.
Similar articles
-
Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components.J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004 Mar;30(2):382-92. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004. PMID: 14979812
-
A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a stroop effect.J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004 Sep;133(3):323-38. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004. PMID: 15355142 Clinical Trial.
-
Selective attention for masked and unmasked emotionally toned stimuli: effects of trait anxiety, state anxiety, and test order.Br J Psychol. 2010 May;101(Pt 2):325-43. doi: 10.1348/000712609X466559. Epub 2009 Aug 25. Br J Psychol. 2010. PMID: 19709474
-
Lexical characteristics of words used in emotional Stroop experiments.Emotion. 2006 Feb;6(1):62-72. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.62. Emotion. 2006. PMID: 16637750 Review.
-
Generalizability of carry-over effects in the emotional Stroop task.Behav Res Ther. 2005 Jun;43(6):715-32. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.003. Behav Res Ther. 2005. PMID: 15890165 Review.
Cited by
-
Testing for the "Blues": Using the Modified Emotional Stroop Task to Assess the Emotional Response of Gorillas.Animals (Basel). 2022 May 6;12(9):1188. doi: 10.3390/ani12091188. Animals (Basel). 2022. PMID: 35565614 Free PMC article.
-
Heterogeneity of performances in several inhibitory control tasks: male rhesus macaques are more easily distracted than females.R Soc Open Sci. 2021 Nov 24;8(11):211564. doi: 10.1098/rsos.211564. eCollection 2021 Nov. R Soc Open Sci. 2021. PMID: 34849250 Free PMC article.
-
Association between olfactory function and inhibition of emotional competing distractors in major depressive disorder.Sci Rep. 2020 Apr 14;10(1):6322. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63416-7. Sci Rep. 2020. PMID: 32286450 Free PMC article.
-
A potential role of the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula in cognitive control, brain rhythms, and event-related potentials.Front Psychol. 2011 Nov 10;2:330. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00330. eCollection 2011. Front Psychol. 2011. PMID: 22084637 Free PMC article.
-
Approach, avoidance, and affect: a meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks.Front Psychol. 2014 May 8;5:378. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00378. eCollection 2014. Front Psychol. 2014. PMID: 24847292 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Miscellaneous