Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2007 Dec;105(3):489-512.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.007. Epub 2006 Dec 19.

Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions?

Affiliations

Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions?

Yuyan Luo et al. Cognition. 2007 Dec.

Abstract

The present research examined whether 12.5-month-old infants take into account what objects an agent knows to be present in a scene when interpreting the agent's actions. In two experiments, the infants watched a female human agent repeatedly reach for and grasp object-A as opposed to object-B on an apparatus floor. Object-B was either (1) visible to the agent through a transparent screen; (2) hidden from the agent (but not the infants) by an opaque screen; or (3) placed by the agent herself behind the opaque screen, so that even though she could no longer see object-B, she knew of its presence there. The infants interpreted the agent's repeated actions toward object-A as revealing a preference for object-A over object-B only when she could see object-B (1) or was aware of its presence in the scene (3). These results indicate that, when watching an agent act on objects in a scene, 12.5-month-old infants keep track of the agent's representation of the physical setting in which these actions occur. If the agent's representation is incomplete, because the agent is ignorant about some aspect of the setting, infants use the agent's representation, rather than their own more complete representation, to interpret the agent's actions.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Schematic drawing of the familiarization and test events shown in the transparent and opaque conditions of Experiment 1.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Mean looking times during the familiarization and test trials of the infants in the transparent and opaque conditions of Experiment 1 and in the preview condition of Experiment 2. Error bars represent standard errors.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Schematic drawing of the preview, familiarization, and test events shown in the preview condition of Experiment 2.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baillargeon R, Li J, Luo Y, Wang S. Under what conditions do infants detect continuity violations? In: Munakata Y, Johnson MH, editors. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and Performance XXI. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 163–188.
    1. Baillargeon R, Li J, Ng W, Yuan S. An account of infants’ physical reasoning. In: Woodward A, Needham A, editors. Learning and the Infant Mind. New York: Oxford University Press; (in press)
    1. Baron-Cohen S. Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1995.
    1. Bíró S, Leslie AM. Infants’ perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. Developmental Science. (in press) - PubMed
    1. Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:958–966. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources