Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2006 Dec;49(6):412-6.

Visual-spatial ability, learning modality and surgical knot tying

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Visual-spatial ability, learning modality and surgical knot tying

Michael G Brandt et al. Can J Surg. 2006 Dec.

Abstract

Introduction: The ability to mentally rotate an object in 3 dimensions has been shown with an individual's score on the Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test. The was to determine whether this Mental Rotations Test could be used to predict performance complex surgical skill - the tying of a 1-handed surgical reef knot. In addition, we learning a spatially complex surgical skill could be achieved more effectively via a computer-based selfdirected learning approach than with a didactic lecture-based teaching method.

Methods: preclerkship medical students at the University of Western Ontario were randomized into computer-based self-directed learning group and a didactic lecture-style learning group. administration of the Mental Rotations Test, the students were taught how to tie a reef knot via the learning modality assigned to their respective group.

Results: Students Mental Rotations Test scores were able to tie more surgical knots in the allocated time Students learning how to tie the surgical knot via the computer-based self-directed showed improvement on their knot tying abilities more rapidly than their didactically trained colleagues.

Conclusion: The ability to mentally rotate an object in 3 dimensions played an important initial learning of a spatially complex surgical technical skill. Our data demonstrated learning was as effective and more practical than traditional lecture-based learning.

Introduction: On a démontré qu'il existe un lien entre la capacité d'une personne à faire tourner mentalement un objet tridimensionnel et le résultat qu'elle obtient au test de rotation mentale de Vandenberg et Kuse. Cette étude visait à déterminer si ce test de rotation mentale pourrait servir à prédire l'exécution d'une technique chirurgicale complexe sur le plan spatial : nouer d'une seule main un nœud plat chirurgical. Nous avons en outre évalué s'il serait possible d'apprendre plus efficacement une technique chirurgicale complexe sur le plan spatial par une méthode informatisée d'apprentissage autodirigé plutôt que par une méthode didactique.

Méthodes: On a affecté par randomisation à deux groupes 60 étudiants en médecine de l'Université Western Ontario qui n'avaient pas encore fait leur stage : un groupe d'apprentissage autodirigé par ordinateur et un groupe d'apprentissage didactique. À la suite de l'administration du test de rotation mentale, on a appris aux étudiants à nouer d'une seule main un nœud plat chirurgical par la technique d'apprentissage affectée à leur groupe respectif.

Résultats: Les étudiants qui ont obtenu des résultats supérieurs au test de rotation mentale ont pu nouer davantage de nœuds chirurgicaux pendant la période prescrite (p < 0,001). Les étudiants qui apprenaient à nouer le nœud chirurgical par la méthode informatisée d'apprentissage autodirigé ont vu leur technique s'améliorer plus rapidement que celle de leurs collègues qui ont suivi une formation didactique.

Conclusion: L'incapacité de produire la rotation mentale d'un objet tridimensionnel a joué un rôle important dans l'apprentissage initial d'une technique chirurgicale complexe sur le plan spatial. Nos données ont démontré que l'apprentissage autogéré était efficace et plus pratique que l'apprentissage didactique traditionnel.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

None
FIG. 1. Sample item from the Mental Rotations Test. This is a sample item appearing on the redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test Version A by Peters and colleagues. 6 The medical students were asked to identify which of the 4 items depicted on the right match the reference object on the left. (Reprinted with permission from Peters et al. 6 ©1995 Elsevier.)
None
FIG. 2. Suture tying board provided by Ethicon Incorporated. Preclerkship medical students were instructed on how to tie a surgical knot across the 2 opposing rubber bands.
None
FIG. 3. Total number of knots tied versus Mental Rotations Test score. A regression plot illustrating the total number of 1-handed surgical reef knots tied during three 10-minute training/repetition sequences versus the Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test scores of preclerkship medical students in both the computer-based self-directed learning group (grey) and the didactically trained group (black). The log-transformed Pearson correlations were r = 0.42, p < 0.05; r = 0.45, p < 0.01) for the computer-based and didactic groups, respectively. Fifty-one points are shown as a result of 9 individuals having equivalent Mental Rotations Test scores and total number of knots tied.
None
FIG. 4. Average number of knots tied per training/repetition sequence. A comparison of the average number of knots tied by preclerkship medical students within each of three 10-minute training/repetition sequences between the computer-based self-directed learning group and the didactically trained group. Error bars correspond to the standard error of the mean shown.
None
FIG. 5. Improvement in the number of knots tied between the first and second training/repetition sequence. A comparison of the improvement (in average number of knots tied by preclerkship medical students) between the first and second training/repetition sequence in both the computer-based self-directed learning group and the didactically trained group. A significant ( p < 0.04) difference was observed. Error bars correspond to the standard error of the mean shown.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Wanzel KR, Ward M, Reznick RK. Teaching the surgical craft: from selection to certification. Curr Probl Surg 2002;39:573-659. - PubMed
    1. Wanzel KR, Hamstra SJ, Anastakis DJ, et al. Effect of visual-spatial ability on learning of spatially-complex surgical skills. Lancet 2002;359:230-1. - PubMed
    1. Wanzel KR, Hamstra SJ, Caminiti MF, et al. Visual-spatial ability correlates with efficiency of hand motion and successful surgical performance. Surgery 2003;134:750-7. - PubMed
    1. Anastakis DJ, Hamstra SJ, Matsumoto ED. Visual-spatial abilities in surgical training. Am J Surg 2000;179:469-71. - PubMed
    1. Vandenberg SG, Kuse AR. Mental rotations: a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept Mot Skills 1978;47:599-604. - PubMed

Publication types