Postconflict third-party affiliation in rooks, Corvus frugilegus
- PMID: 17240341
- DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.025
Postconflict third-party affiliation in rooks, Corvus frugilegus
Abstract
Conflict features in the lives of many animal species and induces social stress mediated by glucocorticoid hormones [1]. Postconflict affiliation, between former opponents (reconciliation) or between former opponents and a bystander (third-party affiliation), has been suggested as a behavioral mechanism for reducing such stress [2], but has been studied almost exclusively in primates [3]. As with many primates, several bird species live in social groups and form affiliative relationships [4]. Do these distantly related animals also use affiliative behavior to offset the costs of conflict? We studied postconflict affiliation in a captive group of rooks. Unlike polygamous primates, monogamous rooks did not reconcile with former opponents. However, we found clear evidence of third-party affiliation after conflicts. Both initiators and targets of aggression engaged in third-party affiliation with a social partner and employed a specific behavior, bill twining, during the postconflict period. Both former aggressors and uninvolved third parties initiated affiliative contacts. Despite the long history of evolutionary divergence, the pattern of third-party affiliation in rooks is strikingly similar to that observed in tolerant primate species. Furthermore, the absence of reconciliation in rooks makes sense in light of the species differences in social systems.
Comment in
-
Animal behavior: conflict management is for the birds.Curr Biol. 2007 Jan 23;17(2):R50-1. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.014. Curr Biol. 2007. PMID: 17240326
Similar articles
-
Conflict management in rooks (Corvus frugilegus): Victims do not display post-conflict affiliation but avoid their former aggressor.Behav Processes. 2020 Oct;179:104198. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104198. Epub 2020 Jul 15. Behav Processes. 2020. PMID: 32679226
-
Post-conflict third-party affiliation in chimpanzees: what's in it for the third party?Am J Primatol. 2009 May;71(5):409-18. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20668. Am J Primatol. 2009. PMID: 19206165
-
Postconflict behavior in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla).Am J Primatol. 2006 Aug;68(8):789-801. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20279. Am J Primatol. 2006. PMID: 16847973
-
Attachment, aggression and affiliation: the role of oxytocin in female social behavior.Biol Psychol. 2008 Jan;77(1):1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.001. Epub 2007 Sep 6. Biol Psychol. 2008. PMID: 17931766 Review.
-
Male-male affiliation and cooperation characterize the social behavior of the large-bodied pitheciids, Chiropotes and Cacajao: A review.Am J Primatol. 2016 May;78(5):550-60. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22404. Epub 2015 Apr 6. Am J Primatol. 2016. PMID: 25845849 Review.
Cited by
-
Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies.Nature. 2009 Nov 5;462(7269):51-7. doi: 10.1038/nature08366. Nature. 2009. PMID: 19890322 Review.
-
Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) reassure others in distress.PeerJ. 2014 Feb 18;2:e278. doi: 10.7717/peerj.278. eCollection 2014. PeerJ. 2014. PMID: 24688856 Free PMC article.
-
What constitutes "social complexity" and "social intelligence" in birds? Lessons from ravens.Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2019;73(1):12. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2607-2. Epub 2019 Jan 19. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2019. PMID: 30930524 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Wild jackdaws respond to their partner's distress, but not with consolation.R Soc Open Sci. 2021 Jun 30;8(6):210253. doi: 10.1098/rsos.210253. eCollection 2021 Jun. R Soc Open Sci. 2021. PMID: 34234956 Free PMC article.
-
Infant attraction: why social bridging matters for female leadership in Tibetan macaques.Curr Zool. 2020 Dec;66(6):635-642. doi: 10.1093/cz/zoaa026. Epub 2020 Jun 13. Curr Zool. 2020. PMID: 33391362 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Research Materials