Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2007 Jun;180(2):333-43.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0861-z. Epub 2007 Jan 26.

Joint-action coordination in transferring objects

Affiliations

Joint-action coordination in transferring objects

Ruud G J Meulenbroek et al. Exp Brain Res. 2007 Jun.

Abstract

Here we report a study of joint-action coordination in transferring objects. Fourteen dyads were asked to repeatedly reposition a cylinder in a shared workspace without using dialogue. Variations in task constraints concerned the size of the two target regions in which the cylinder had to be (re)positioned and the size and weight of the transferred cylinder. Movements of the wrist, index finger and thumb of both actors were recorded by means of a 3D motion-tracking system. Data analyses focused on the interpersonal transfer of lifting-height and movement-speed variations. Whereas the analyses of variance did not reveal any interpersonal transfer effects targeted data comparisons demonstrated that the actor who fetched the cylinder from where the other actor had put it was systematically less surprised by cylinder-weight changes than the actor who was first confronted with such changes. In addition, a moderate, accuracy-constraint independent adaptation to each other's movement speed was found. The current findings suggest that motor resonance plays only a moderate role in collaborative motor control and confirm the independency between sensorimotor and cognitive processing of action-related information.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Top view of experimental setup depicting the experimental task. The dashed squares represent the starting areas for the (right) hand movements of both actors. At the filled circle a cylinder was positioned which had to be picked up by the putting-actor, repositioned in the central circular target area where the fetching-actor was to pick it up and place it in the target circle in the vicinity of her own starting area
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Kinematics derived from position data of infrared-light emitting diode (IRED) on tip of the thumb, tip of the index finger and wrist as obtained in a prototypical trial. Green (light grey) functions are from the putting-actor, red (dark grey) functions are from the fetching-actor. The top-left graph shows the tangential wrist-velocity time functions, the top-right graph a 3D rendering of finger-thumb displacements, the bottom-left graph shows the aperture-time functions, and the bottom-right graph shows the height (in cm above the table surface) of the midpoint between the thumb and index-finger IREDs
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Means and standard errors of the lifting height (in cm) as a function of role (putting vs. fetching), object diameter (small vs. large) and object weight (light vs. heavy) for repetitions 1, 2 and 3 separately
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Means and standard errors of the lifting height (in cm) of the putting-actor and the fetching-actor. Left-hand graph: whenever the putting-actor showed a lifting-height increase from trial i − 1 to trial i due to an unexpected mass reduction between these trials, the fetching-actor also showed this surprise effect but less strong. Trials i − 1 were the third trials of the trial blocks; trials i were the first trials of the subsequent trial blocks. Right-hand graph: similarly, whenever the putting-actor showed a lifting-height decrease from trial i − 1 to trial i due to an unexpected mass increase between these trials, the fetching-actor also showed this surprise effect, but again, not as strong as the putting-actor did
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Example of the linear regression analysis between the lifting-height data generated by the putter and fetcher of 1 of the 14 dyads participating in the study. The data concern the first trial within trial blocks of three repetitions of the experimental conditions. The regression analyses revealed the robustness of the observed reduction of the size–weight illusion due to movement observation in the dyads studied. The dashed line with slope = 1 represents the situation in which the lifting height of the fetcher would equal that of the putter
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Means and standard errors of the mean wrist speed (in cm/s) as a function of role (putting vs. fetching) and target area size (S = Small, L = Large; p = put, f = fetch). The labels with subscript p reflect the size of the target area of the putting-actor and labels with subscript f reflect the size of the target area of the fetching-actor
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
Means and standard errors of the wrist speed (in cm/s) of the putting-actor and the fetching-actor. Left-hand graph: whenever the putting-actor showed a task-unrelated speed increase from trial i − 1 to trial i, the fetching-actor also showed such a speed increase. The trials involved concerned the second and third trials of the trial blocks in which no task conditions changed. Right-hand graph: similarly, whenever the putting-actor showed a task-unrelated speed decrease from trial i − 1 to trial i, the fetching-actor followed this speed decrease
Fig. 8
Fig. 8
Example of the linear regression analysis between the wrist-speed data generated by the putter and fetcher of 1 of the 14 dyads participating in the study. The data concern the second and third trials within trial blocks of three repetitions of the experimental conditions. The regression analyzes revealed the robustness of the observed, task-constraint independent covariation of movement speed between the two actors. The dashed line with slope = 1 represents the situation in which the wrist speed of the fetcher would equal that of the putter

References

    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0096-1523.22.1.213', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.22.1.213'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '8742263', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8742263/'}]}
    2. Amazeen EL, Turvey MT (1996) Weight perception and the haptic size–weight illusion are functions of the inertia tensor. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:213–232 - PubMed
    1. None
    2. Charpentier A (1891) Analyse experimentale de quelgues elements de la sensation de poids. Arch Physiol Norm Pathol 3:122–135
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10402679', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10402679/'}]}
    2. Chartrand TL, Bargh J (1999) The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76:893–910 - PubMed
    1. Csibra G (2005) Mirror neurons and action observation. Is simulation involved? Publication of the European Science Foundation, retrieved 2 May 2006 from http://www.interdisciplines.org/mirror
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/j.neunet.2006.02.004', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2006.02.004'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '16618535', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16618535/'}]}
    2. Cuijpers RH, van Schie HT, Koppen M, Erlhagen W, Bekkering H (2006) Goal and means in action observation: a computational approach. Neural Netw 19:311–322 - PubMed

Publication types