Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2007 May 16;91(1):1-8.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.005. Epub 2007 Jan 18.

Partner preference in male hamsters: steroids, sexual experience and chemosensory cues

Affiliations

Partner preference in male hamsters: steroids, sexual experience and chemosensory cues

Cortney L Ballard et al. Physiol Behav. .

Abstract

This study investigated the effects of gonadal steroids on sexual motivation in male Syrian hamsters, using partner preference as a model. Male hamsters were assigned to 5 groups: control (n=4), Intact-->Orchx (n=8), Orchx-->Orchx+T (n=7), olfactory bulbectomy (BulbX, n=5), and vomeronasal organ lesion (VnoX, n=8). Each male was tested for partner preference before and after sexual experience. Unlike rats, sexually-inexperienced gonad-intact male hamsters preferred the receptive female to a stimulus male. However, sexual experience did not enhance preference for the stimulus female. Castration (Orchx) reduced sexual motivation: Orchx males showed no significant preference for the stimulus female. Subsequently, intact males were castrated (Intact-->Orchx) and Orchx males received a testosterone implant (Orchx-->Orchx+T) to determine the time course of gonadal hormones on partner preference and mating behavior. Partner preference changed significantly in both groups within 6 weeks. In Intact-->Orchx males, preference for the stimulus female decreased while Orchx-->Orchx+T males increased their preference for the stimulus female. However, significant changes in mating behavior preceded the alterations in partner preference. Chemosensory cues are also important for partner preference. After BulbX, preference for the stimulus female significantly decreased. However, VnoX failed to block partner preference. These results show that partner preference may be even more dependent on testosterone than is sexual behavior. Furthermore, while chemosensory cues are essential for sexual motivation, the vomeronasal organ is not required for partner preference.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Top: Schematic of apparatus for testing partner preference in male hamsters. Middle: Timeline for partner preference and copulation tests in male hamsters before and after sexual experience and castration (orchX). Bottom: Time (mean±SEM) with an estrous female (black bars) or male stimulus animal (white bars) in 10-minute partner preference tests before (n=12, shaded area) and after castration (n=8) in male Syrian hamsters. Asterisk indicates significant effect of time post-castration. Gray symbols indicate behavior of gonad-intact control males tested at the same intervals.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Top: Timeline for partner preference and copulation tests in castrated (OrchX) male hamsters before and after sexual experience and testosterone replacement (OrchX+T). Bottom: Time (mean±SEM) with an estrous female (black bars) or male stimulus animal (white bars) in 10-minute partner preference tests before and after (shaded area) testosterone replacement in male Syrian hamsters (n=8).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Top: Timeline for partner preference and copulation tests in male hamsters before and after olfactory bulbectomy (BulbX) or vomeronasal organ removal (VnoX). Bottom: Time (mean±SEM) with an estrous female (black bars) or male stimulus animal (white bars) in 10-minute partner preference tests before and after surgery or sexual experience in male Syrian hamsters (n=8/group). Asterisk indicates significant effect of BulbX on partner preference.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Copulatory behaviors (mean±SEM) in male Syrian hamsters (n=8) before (black symbols, shaded) and at 5 and 7 weeks after castration (white symbols). Numbers in parentheses reflect the number of animals expressing behavior at each time point.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Copulatory behaviors (mean±SEM) in castrated male Syrian hamsters (n=8) before (white symbols) and at 5 and 7 weeks after testosterone replacement (black symbols, shaded). Numbers in parentheses reflect the number of animals expressing behavior at each time point.

References

    1. Everitt BJ. Sexual motivation: A neural and behavioural analysis of the mechanisms underlying appetitive and copulatory responses of male rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1990;14:217–232. - PubMed
    1. Sisk CL, Zehr JL. Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2005;26(3–4):163–174. - PubMed
    1. Pospichal MW, Karp JD, Powers JB. Influence of daylength on male hamster sexual behavior: masking effects of testosterone. Physiol Behav. 1991;49(3):417–422. - PubMed
    1. Gregory E, Engel K, Pfaff DW. Male hamster preference for odors of female hamster vaginal discharges: studies of experiential and hormonal determinants. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1975;89:442–446. - PubMed
    1. Steel E, Keverne EB. Effect of female odour on male hamsters mediated by the vomeronasal organ. Physiol Behav. 1985;35:195–200. - PubMed

Publication types