Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2007 Sep 4;182(2):155-65.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.011. Epub 2007 Feb 16.

Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals

Affiliations
Review

Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals

John D Newman. Behav Brain Res. .

Abstract

Crying is a universal vocalization in human infants, as well as in the infants of other mammals. Little is known about the neural structures underlying cry production, or the circuitry that mediates a caregiver's response to cry sounds. In this review, the specific structures known or suspected to be involved in this circuit are identified, along with neurochemical systems and hormones for which evidence suggests a role in responding to infants and infant cries. In addition, evidence that crying elicits parental responses in different mammals is presented. An argument is made for including 'crying' as a functional category in the vocal repertoire of all mammalian infants (and the adults of some species). The prevailing neural model for crying production considers forebrain structures to be dispensable. However, evidence for the anterior cingulate gyrus in cry production, and this structure along with the amygdala and some other forebrain areas in responding to cries is presented.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Spectrographic examples of infant cries in two primate species. These illustrate the two types of infant cry in mammals. Left) Click emitted by Garnett’s greater bushbaby (Otolemur garnetti) after being deposited on a branch by its mother at 4 days of age. Right) Coo (‘tonal’ cry) following brief maternal separation in rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) at 14 days of age.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Almond RE, Brown GR, Keverne EB. Suppression of prolactin does not reduce infant care by parentally inexperienced male common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) . Horm Behav. 2006;49:673–680. - PubMed
    1. Barnet A, Bazelon M, Zapella M. Visual and auditory function in an hydranencephalic infant. Brain Res. 1966;2:351–360. - PubMed
    1. Barr RG, Hopkins B, Green JA, editors. Clinics in Developmental Medicine no. 152. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge United Kingdom: 2000. Crying as a Sign, Symptom and Signal.
    1. Bauman MD, Lavenex P, Mason WA, Capitanio JP, Amaral DG. The development of mother-infant interactions after neonatal amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci. 2004;24:711–721. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Becker ML. Vocal communication in the small-eared bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii): Morphology, sound structure, and social context (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Memphis, 2000) Diss Abstr Int-B. 2001;61(11):6169.