Depressive realism and the effect of intertrial interval on judgements of zero, positive, and negative contingencies
- PMID: 17366312
- DOI: 10.1080/17470210601002595
Depressive realism and the effect of intertrial interval on judgements of zero, positive, and negative contingencies
Abstract
In three experiments we tested how the spacing of trials during acquisition of zero, positive, and negative response-outcome contingencies differentially affected depressed and nondepressed students' judgements. Experiment 1 found that nondepressed participants' judgements of zero contingencies increased with longer intertrial intervals (ITIs) but not simply longer procedure durations. Depressed groups' judgements were not sensitive to either manipulation, producing an effect known as depressive realism only with long ITIs. Experiments 2 and 3 tested predictions of Cheng's (1997) Power PC theory and the Rescorla-Wagner (1972) model, that the increase in context exposure experienced during the ITI might influence judgements most with negative contingencies and least with positive contingencies. Results suggested that depressed people were less sensitive to differences in contingency and contextual exposure. We propose that a context-processing difference between depressed and nondepressed people removes any objective notion of "realism" that was originally employed to explain the depressive realism effect (Alloy & Abramson, 1979).
Similar articles
-
Depressive realism and outcome density bias in contingency judgments: the effect of the context and intertrial interval.J Exp Psychol Gen. 2005 Feb;134(1):10-22. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.10. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2005. PMID: 15702960
-
The effects of cognitive load during intertrial intervals on judgements of control: The role of working memory and contextual learning.Acta Psychol (Amst). 2016 Nov;171:47-56. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.09.005. Epub 2016 Sep 20. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2016. PMID: 27662425
-
Metacognition and depressive realism: evidence for the level-of-depression account.Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2011 Sep;16(5):461-72. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2011.557921. Epub 2011 May 24. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2011. PMID: 21390922
-
The sad truth about depressive realism.Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2007 Mar;60(3):482-95. doi: 10.1080/17470210601002686. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2007. PMID: 17366313 Review.
-
Depressive realism: a meta-analytic review.Clin Psychol Rev. 2012 Aug;32(6):496-509. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.05.004. Epub 2012 May 22. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012. PMID: 22717337 Review.
Cited by
-
Illusions of causality: how they bias our everyday thinking and how they could be reduced.Front Psychol. 2015 Jul 2;6:888. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00888. eCollection 2015. Front Psychol. 2015. PMID: 26191014 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Interactive effects of the probability of the cue and the probability of the outcome on the overestimation of null contingency.Learn Behav. 2013 Dec;41(4):333-40. doi: 10.3758/s13420-013-0108-8. Learn Behav. 2013. PMID: 23529636
-
5-HT modulation by acute tryptophan depletion of human instrumental contingency judgements.Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011 Feb;213(2-3):615-23. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1934-4. Epub 2010 Jul 15. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011. PMID: 20631992 Free PMC article.
-
Why are Some Games More Addictive than Others: The Effects of Timing and Payoff on Perseverance in a Slot Machine Game.Front Psychol. 2016 Feb 2;7:46. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00046. eCollection 2016. Front Psychol. 2016. PMID: 26869955 Free PMC article.
-
Context and time in causal learning: contingency and mood dependent effects.PLoS One. 2013 May 15;8(5):e64063. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064063. Print 2013. PLoS One. 2013. PMID: 23691147 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Medical