When does action resist visual illusion? The effect of Müller-Lyer stimuli on reflexive and voluntary saccades
- PMID: 17372725
- DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-0927-y
When does action resist visual illusion? The effect of Müller-Lyer stimuli on reflexive and voluntary saccades
Abstract
The primate visual cortex exhibits two anatomically distinct pathways (dorsal and ventral). According to the "two visual systems hypothesis" (TVSH) of Milner and Goodale (The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1995), this anatomical distinction corresponds to a functional division of labor between vision-for-action (dorsal) and vision-for-perception (ventral). This proposal is supported by evidence that, in healthy volunteers, perceptual responses are affected by visual illusions, whereas motor responses to the same illusion-inducing stimuli are not. However, previously we have shown that the amplitude of saccadic eye movements is modified by the Müller-Lyer illusion in a similar manner as perceptual responses. Here we extend this finding to reflexive and voluntary (memory-guided) saccades. We show that both types of saccade can be strongly affected by the illusion. In our studies, the effect on reflexive saccades was comparable to that usually observed with verbal reports (an effect size of 22 +/- 8%), whereas the effect on voluntary saccades was smaller (11 +/- 11%). In addition, both types of saccade provide evidence for the scaling bias usually observed in perceptual responses. We suggest that previous studies may have employed methods that generally reduced the effect of the illusion. Interpretations of dissociations between reflexive and voluntary saccades in terms of the TVSH appear to be premature.
Similar articles
-
Time course of the effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on saccades and perceptual judgments.J Vis. 2014 Jan 6;14(1):4. doi: 10.1167/14.1.4. J Vis. 2014. PMID: 24396047
-
Differential effects of the Müller-Lyer illusion on reflexive and voluntary saccades.J Vis. 2003 Dec 4;3(11):751-60. doi: 10.1167/3.11.9. J Vis. 2003. PMID: 14765958
-
The effect of Kanizsa's compression illusion on reflexive saccades.Exp Brain Res. 2006 Nov;175(4):764-8. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0741-y. Epub 2006 Oct 19. Exp Brain Res. 2006. PMID: 17051374
-
A metanalysis of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: no general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action.Vision Res. 2010 Dec;50(24):2671-82. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.016. Epub 2010 Sep 19. Vision Res. 2010. PMID: 20858510 Review.
-
When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review.Neuropsychologia. 2009 May;47(6):1421-33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.031. Epub 2008 Nov 19. Neuropsychologia. 2009. PMID: 19059422 Review.
Cited by
-
The reduction of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion saccade amplitude by classic adaptation.Iperception. 2010;1(2):95-102. doi: 10.1068/i0395. Epub 2010 Nov 5. Iperception. 2010. PMID: 23396270 Free PMC article.
-
Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence.Exp Brain Res. 2008 Jan;184(1):61-70. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1081-2. Epub 2007 Aug 24. Exp Brain Res. 2008. PMID: 17717656 Free PMC article.
-
Stimulus-driven saccades are characterized by an invariant undershooting bias: no evidence for a range effect.Exp Brain Res. 2013 Oct;230(2):165-74. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3640-z. Epub 2013 Jul 25. Exp Brain Res. 2013. PMID: 23884554
-
Keeping a target in memory does not increase the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccades.Exp Brain Res. 2016 Apr;234(4):977-83. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4520-5. Epub 2015 Dec 21. Exp Brain Res. 2016. PMID: 26686530 Free PMC article.
-
Rapid eye movements to a virtual target are biased by illusory context in the Poggendorff figure.Exp Brain Res. 2015 Jul;233(7):1993-2000. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4263-3. Epub 2015 Apr 26. Exp Brain Res. 2015. PMID: 25912606 Free PMC article.
References
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources