Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2007 Sep;47(19):2506-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.06.012. Epub 2007 Aug 10.

Losing sight of the bigger picture: peripheral field loss compresses representations of space

Affiliations

Losing sight of the bigger picture: peripheral field loss compresses representations of space

Francesca C Fortenbaugh et al. Vision Res. 2007 Sep.

Abstract

Three experiments examine how the peripheral visual field (PVF) mediates the development of spatial representations. In Experiment 1 participants learned and were tested on statue locations in a virtual environment while their field-of-view (FOV) was restricted to 40 degrees , 20 degrees , 10 degrees , or 0 degrees (diam). As FOV decreased, overall placement errors, estimated distances, and angular offsets increased. Experiment 2 showed large compressions but no effect of FOV for perceptual estimates of statue locations. Experiment 3 showed an association between FOV size and proprioception influence. These results suggest the PVF provides important global spatial information used in the development of spatial representations.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. The virtual environment
The top panel shows a first-person view of one end of the environment. The bottom panel shows a top-down view of the environment. The starting position is the red and white bull’s-eye and the two black rectangles represent support columns that were present in the real and virtual environments.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Behavioral measures for Experiment 1
Mean estimated locations of the six statues for each of the four FOV conditions tested. The black square is the starting position and the black circles represent the true statue locations. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Behavioral measures for Experiment 1
(a) Mean distance errors in meters as a function of FOV and Statue Distance (Near, Far). Positive y-values indicate an overestimation of distance and negative y-values indicate distances were underestimated. (b) Mean absolute angular offsets in degrees as a function of FOV and Statue Distance. Error bars represent ±1 SEM and FOV is in degrees of visual angle.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Behavioral measures for Experiment 1
(a) Mean distance errors in meters as a function of FOV and Statue Distance (Near, Far). Positive y-values indicate an overestimation of distance and negative y-values indicate distances were underestimated. (b) Mean absolute angular offsets in degrees as a function of FOV and Statue Distance. Error bars represent ±1 SEM and FOV is in degrees of visual angle.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Gaze strategies for Experiment 1
Mean proportion of fixations made to each of the five object categories (statues, walls, ground, columns, and sky) as a function of FOV for the Learning Phase. Error bars represent ±1 SEM and FOV is in degrees of visual angle.
Figure 5
Figure 5. Experiment 2
(a) Mean distance errors in meters for the perceptual estimates as a function of FOV and Statue Distance. (b) Mean height errors in meters for the perceptual estimates as a function of FOV and Statue Distance. (c) Mean errors in meters for the perceptual estimates of distances between pairs of statues as a function of FOV. (d) Mean placement errors in meters for the three blind-walks as a function of Statue Distance and FOV. Error bars represent ±1 SEM and FOV is in degrees of visual angle.
Figure 6
Figure 6. Experiment 3
Mean distance errors in meters as a function of Statue Distance and FOV for the Stand and Walk conditions, respectively. Error bars represent ±1 SEM and FOV is in degrees of visual angle.

References

    1. Aivar MP, Hayhoe MM, Chizk CL, Mruczek REB. Spatial memory and saccadic targeting in a natural task. Journal of Vision. 2005;5(3):177–193. - PubMed
    1. Baizer JS, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;17:166–190. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Banks MS, Sekuler AB, Anderson SJ. Peripheral spatial vision: Limits imposed by optics, photoreceptors, and receptor pooling. Journal of the Optical Society of America: A. 1991;8:1775–1787. - PubMed
    1. Bishop PO, Henry GH. Spatial vision. Annual Review of Psychology. 1971;22:119–160. - PubMed
    1. Brockmole JR, Irwin DE. Eye movements and the integration of visual memory and visual perception. Perception & Psychophysics. 2005;67(3):495–512. - PubMed

Publication types