Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Clinical Trial
. 2007 Sep 26;2(9):e943.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000943.

Causal inference in multisensory perception

Affiliations
Clinical Trial

Causal inference in multisensory perception

Konrad P Körding et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Perceptual events derive their significance to an animal from their meaning about the world, that is from the information they carry about their causes. The brain should thus be able to efficiently infer the causes underlying our sensory events. Here we use multisensory cue combination to study causal inference in perception. We formulate an ideal-observer model that infers whether two sensory cues originate from the same location and that also estimates their location(s). This model accurately predicts the nonlinear integration of cues by human subjects in two auditory-visual localization tasks. The results show that indeed humans can efficiently infer the causal structure as well as the location of causes. By combining insights from the study of causal inference with the ideal-observer approach to sensory cue combination, we show that the capacity to infer causal structure is not limited to conscious, high-level cognition; it is also performed continually and effortlessly in perception.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. The causal inference model.
Left: One cause can be responsible for both cues. In this case the visually perceived position xV will be the common position s perturbed by visual noise with width σV and the auditory perceived position will be the common position perturbed by auditory noise with width σA. Right: Alternatively, two distinct causes may be relevant, decoupling the problem into two independent estimation problems. The causal inference model infers the probability of a causal structure with a common cause (left, C = 1) versus the causal structure with two independent causes (right, C = 2) and then derives optimal predictions from this. We introduce a single variable C which determines which sub-model generates the data.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Combination of visual and auditory cues.
a) The experimental paradigm is shown. In each trial a visual and an auditory stimulus is presented simultaneously and subjects report both the position of the perceived visual and the position of the perceived auditory stimuli by button presses. b) The influence of vision on the perceived position of an auditory stimulus in the center is shown. Different colors correspond to the visual stimulus at different locations (sketched in warm to cold colors from the left to the right). The unimodal auditory case is shown in gray. c) The averaged responses of the subjects (solid lines) are shown along with the predictions of the ideal observer (broken lines) for each of the 35 stimulus conditions. These plot show how often on average which button was pressed in each of the conditions. d) The model responses from c) are plotted with the human responses from c). e) The average auditory bias formula image, i.e. the influence of deviations of the visual position on the perceived auditory position is shown as a function of the spatial disparity (solid line) along with the model prediction (dashed line).
Figure 3
Figure 3. Reports of causal inference.
a) The relative frequency of subjects reporting one cause (black) is shown (reprinted with permission from [15]) with the prediction of the causal inference model (red). b) The bias, i.e. the influence of vision on the perceived auditory position is shown (gray and black). The predictions of the model are shown in red. c) A schematic illustration explaining the finding of negative biases. Blue and black dots represent the perceived visual and auditory stimuli, respectively. In the pink area people perceive a common cause.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Hatfield GC. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press; 1990. The natural and the normative theories of spatial perception from Kant to Helmholtz.
    1. Thurlow WR, Jack CE. Certain determinants of the “ventriloquism effect”. Percept Mot Skills. 1973;36:1171–1184. - PubMed
    1. Warren DH, Welch RB, McCarthy TJ. The role of visual-auditory “compellingness” in the ventriloquism effect: implications for transitivity among the spatial senses. Percept Psychophys. 1981;30:557–564. - PubMed
    1. Jacobs RA. Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth. Vision Res. 1999;39:3621–3629. - PubMed
    1. van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Gon JJ. Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:1355–1364. - PubMed

Publication types