Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2007 Nov;13(6):953-60.
doi: 10.1017/S1355617707071512.

Effect of memory impairment on training outcomes in ACTIVE

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Effect of memory impairment on training outcomes in ACTIVE

Frederick W Unverzagt et al. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007 Nov.

Abstract

Cognitive training improves mental abilities in older adults, but the trainability of persons with memory impairment is unclear. We conducted a subgroup analysis of subjects in the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) trial to examine this issue. ACTIVE enrolled 2802 non-demented, community-dwelling adults aged 65 years and older and randomly assigned them to one of four groups: Memory training, reasoning training, speed-of-processing training, or no-contact control. For this study, participants were defined as memory-impaired if baseline Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) sum recall score was 1.5 SD or more below predicted AVLT sum recall score from a regression-derived formula using age, education, ethnicity, and vocabulary from all subjects at baseline. Assessments were taken at baseline (BL), post-test, first annual (A1), and second annual (A2) follow-up. One hundred and ninety-three subjects were defined as memory-impaired and 2580 were memory-normal. Training gain as a function memory status (impaired vs. normal) was compared in a mixed effects model. Results indicated that memory-impaired participants failed to benefit from Memory training but did show normal training gains after reasoning and speed training. Memory function appears to mediate response to structured cognitive interventions in older adults.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Study design.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Cognitive training effects as a function of participant's memory status (normal vs. impaired). Panel A: Memory trained participants on the memory composite outcome measure. Panel B: Reasoning trained participants on the reasoning composite outcome measure. Panel C: Speed trained participants on the speed composite outcome measure. PT = Immediate post-test assessment, A1 = first annual assessment, A2 = second annual assessment.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Smith DM, Tennstedt SL, Unverzagt FW, Willis SL, Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study Group Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2002;288:2271–2281. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs DS. Age and Visual-Search—Expanding the Useful Field of View. Journal of the Optical Society of America A-Optics Image Science and Vision. 1988;5:2210–2219. - PubMed
    1. Baltes PB, Willis SL. Plasticity and enhancement of intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State's Adult Development and Enrichment Project (ADEPT) In: Craik F, Trehub S, editors. Aging and Cognitive Processes. Plenum Press; New York: 1982. pp. 353–390.
    1. Belleville S, Gilbert B, Fontaine F, Gagnon L, Menard E, Gauthier S. Improvement of episodic memory in persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment and healthy older adults: Evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2006;22:486–499. - PubMed
    1. Blom G. Statistical Estimates and Transformed Beta Variables. John Wiley & Sons; New York: 1958.

Publication types