Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Feb;30(2):473-83.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.20523.

Strengthening of laterality of verbal and visuospatial functions during childhood and adolescence

Affiliations

Strengthening of laterality of verbal and visuospatial functions during childhood and adolescence

Regula Everts et al. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009 Feb.

Abstract

Cognitive functions in the child's brain develop in the context of complex adaptive processes, determined by genetic and environmental factors. Little is known about the cerebral representation of cognitive functions during development. In particular, knowledge about the development of right hemispheric (RH) functions is scarce. Considering the dynamics of brain development, localization and lateralization of cognitive functions must be expected to change with age. Twenty healthy subjects (8.6-20.5 years) were examined with fMRI and neuropsychological tests. All participants completed two fMRI tasks known to activate left hemispheric (LH) regions (language tasks) and two tasks known to involve predominantly RH areas (visual search tasks). A laterality index (LI) was computed to determine the asymmetry of activation. Group analysis revealed unilateral activation of the LH language circuitry during language tasks while visual search tasks induced a more widespread RH activation pattern in frontal, superior temporal, and occipital areas. Laterality of language increased between the ages of 8-20 in frontal (r = 0.392, P = 0.049) and temporal (r = 0.387, P = 0.051) areas. The asymmetry of visual search functions increased in frontal (r = -0.525, P = 0.009) and parietal (r = -0.439, P = 0.027) regions. A positive correlation was found between Verbal-IQ and the LI during a language task (r = 0.585, P = 0.028), while visuospatial skills correlated with LIs of visual search (r = -0.621, P = 0.018). To summarize, cognitive development is accompanied by changes in the functional representation of neuronal circuitries, with a strengthening of lateralization not only for LH but also for RH functions. Our data show that age and performance, independently, account for the increases of laterality with age.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Activation (left) and contrast (right) condition of the two language tasks (above) and the visual search tasks (below). Original tasks were presented in color.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Group analysis of children (n = 20) during the two visual search tasks (left) and the two language tasks (right). Group analysis: n = 20, random effect analysis, P < 0.05, FWE‐corrected, extended threshold k > 50 voxels. One child (animal search task) or two children (Rey search task) were excluded from analysis due to movement >3mm. See text for details regarding the identified activation sites. [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at www.interscience.wiley.com.]
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean laterality indices (LI) over all brain regions of the four paradigms. Left lateralization was assumed at laterality index >0.2, right lateralization was assumed at laterality index <−0.2.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Relation between age at examination and laterality indices for the frontal and parietal region during the Rey search task (n = 19). * indicates significant correlation at the P < 0.05 level (partial 2‐tailed Pearson correlation controlling for visuo‐spatial performance, copy of the Rey Figure). One child with RH language is not included in the analysis.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Relation between age at examination and laterality indices for the frontal and temporal region in the vowel detection task (n = 19). * indicates significant correlation at the P < 0.05 level (partial 2‐tailed Pearson correlation controlling for Verbal‐IQ). One child with RH language is not included in the analysis.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Regression analyses according to age at examination. Regression: simple, P > 0.001, uncorrected. The regression analysis shows areas that are more active in older children compared to younger ones. [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at www.interscience.wiley.com.]

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adcock JE,Wise RG,Oxbury JM,Oxbury SM,Matthews PM ( 2003): Quantitative fMRI assessment of the differences in lateralization of language‐related brain activation in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage 18: 423–438. - PubMed
    1. Binder JR,Rao SM,Hammeke TA,Frost JA,Bandettini PA,Jesmanowicz A,Hyde JS ( 1995): Lateralized human brain language systems demonstrated by task subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 52: 593–601. - PubMed
    1. Binder JR,Swanson SJ,Hammeke TA,Morris GL,Mueller WM,Fischer M,Benbadis S,Frost JA,Rao SM,Haughton VM ( 1996): Determination of language dominance using functional MRI: A comparison with the Wada test. Neurology 46: 978–984. - PubMed
    1. Cabeza R,Nyberg L ( 2000): Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 1–47. - PubMed
    1. Cuenod CA,Bookheimer SY,Hertz‐Pannier L,Zeffiro TA,Theodore WH,Le Bihan D ( 1995): Functional MRI during word generation, using conventional equipment: A potential tool for language localization in the clinical environment. Neurology 45: 1821–1827. - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms