Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Sep;18(9):2132-40.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm240. Epub 2008 Feb 5.

COMT val158met genotype affects recruitment of neural mechanisms supporting fluid intelligence

Affiliations

COMT val158met genotype affects recruitment of neural mechanisms supporting fluid intelligence

Sonia J Bishop et al. Cereb Cortex. 2008 Sep.

Abstract

Fluid intelligence (g(f)) influences performance across many cognitive domains. It is affected by both genetic and environmental factors. Tasks tapping g(f) activate a network of brain regions including the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC), the presupplementary motor area/anterior cingulate cortex (pre-SMA/ACC), and the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). In line with the "intermediate phenotype" approach, we assessed effects of a polymorphism (val(158)met) in the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene on activity within this network and on actual task performance during spatial and verbal g(f) tasks. COMT regulates catecholaminergic signaling in prefrontal cortex. The val(158) allele is associated with higher COMT activity than the met(158) allele. Twenty-two volunteers genotyped for the COMT val(158)met polymorphism completed high and low g(f) versions of spatial and verbal problem-solving tasks. Our results showed a positive effect of COMT val allele load upon the blood oxygen level-dependent response in LPFC, pre-SMA/ACC, and IPS during high g(f) versus low g(f) task performance in both spatial and verbal domains. These results indicate an influence of the COMT val(158)met polymorphism upon the neural circuitry supporting g(f). The behavioral effects of val allele load differed inside and outside the scanner, consistent with contextual modulation of the relation between COMT val(158)met genotype and g(f) task performance.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Example test items for each task. Each item consisted of 4 display elements (drawings or letter sets), and the task was to identify the element that in some sense mismatched or differed from the others. Materials for the high gf tasks were adapted with permission from a standard nonverbal test of gf, Cattell Culture Fair, Scale 2 Form A and Scale 3 (Institute for Personality and Ability Testing 1973) and a standard letter-based problem-solving task, Letter Sets from the ETS kit of factor-referenced tests (Ekstrom et al. 1976). The high gf loading of these tasks was previously established (Wothke et al. 1991; Duncan et al. 2000). The low gf items were structurally similar but with a minimal problem-solving component. Participants were asked to select the only nonidentical item for the low gf spatial task and to select the string not in alphabetic order for the low gf verbal task.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Neural activation associated with high gf versus low gf task performance. (A) Significant activations at a whole-brain fdr threshold of P < 0.05, rendered onto the canonical T1-weighted brain image of SPM99. (i) Spatial high gf − spatial low gf. (ii) Verbal high gf − verbal low gf. (B) Activation peaks. For significant (fdr, P < 0.05) clusters of any size that overlap the a priori specified ROIs, the table gives peak voxel within the ROI. For other brain areas, peaks are reported only for clusters of 20 or more significant voxels. OFC: orbitofrontal cortex.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Correlation between number of COMT val alleles possessed (0: met/met, 1: val/met, and 2: val/val) and activation for high gf versus low gf conditions for the spatial problem-solving task (A: right DLPFC, B: dorsal ACC, C: pre-SMA, and D: left IPS) and the verbal problem-solving task (E: right DLPFC, F: right FO/AI, G: pre-SMA, and H: left IPS). Left: Activations thresholded at P < 0.05 svc are overlaid on the canonical T1 SPM99 brain. Right: Individual BOLD responses were extracted from the voxel with the highest Z value inside the ROI and plotted against number of COMT val alleles possessed.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ackerman PL, Beier ME, Boyle MO. Working memory and intelligence: the same or different constructs? Psychol Bull. 2005;131:30–60. - PubMed
    1. Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:362–368. - PubMed
    1. Barnett JH, Jones PB, Robbins TW, Muller U. Effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism on executive function: a meta-analysis of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:502–509. - PubMed
    1. Bentivoglio M, Morelli M. The organization and circuits of mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons and the distribution of dopamine receptors in the brain. In: Dunnett SB, Bentivoglio M, Bjorklund A, Hokfelt T, editors. Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy. Vol. 21. Dopamine. Amsterdam (NL): Elsevier; 2005. pp. 1–107.
    1. Bertolino A, Blasi G, Latorre V, Rubino V, Rampino A, Sinibaldi L, Caforio G, Petruzzella V, Pizzuti A, Scarabino T, et al. Additive effects of genetic variation in dopamine regulating genes on working memory cortical activity in human brain. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3918–3922. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

Substances