Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Apr;65(4):421-9.
doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.4.421.

A longitudinal twin study of fears from middle childhood to early adulthood: evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome

Affiliations

A longitudinal twin study of fears from middle childhood to early adulthood: evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome

Kenneth S Kendler et al. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008 Apr.

Abstract

Context: While the nature of common fears changes over development, we do not know whether genetic effects on fear-proneness are developmentally stable or developmentally dynamic.

Objective: To determine the temporal pattern of genetic and environmental effects on the level of intensity of common fears.

Design: Prospective, 4-wave longitudinal twin study. Structural modeling was performed with Mx.

Setting: General community.

Participants: Two thousand four hundred ninety twins and their parents from the Swedish Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development.

Main outcome measure: The level of parent- and/or self-reported fears obtained at ages 8 to 9, 13 to 14, 16 to 17, and 19 to 20 years.

Results: Thirteen questionnaire items formed 3 distinct fear factors: situational, animal, and blood/injury. For all 3 fears, the best-fit model revealed developmentally dynamic effects and, in particular, evidence for both genetic attenuation and innovation. That is, genetic factors influencing fear intensity at age 8 to 9 years decline substantially in importance over time. Furthermore, new sets of genetic risk factors impacting fear intensity "come on line" in early adolescence, late adolescence, and early adulthood. As the twins aged, the influence of the shared environment declined and unique environment increased. No sex effects were found for situational fears while for animal and blood/injury fears, genetic factors in males and females were correlated but not identical. Shared environmental factors were both more important and more stable for animal fears than for situational or blood/injury fears.

Conclusions: Genetic effects on fear are developmentally dynamic from middle childhood to young adulthood. As children age, familial-environmental influences on fears decline in importance.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
The model used in these analyses presented for 1 source of liability, such as additive genetic effects. The model contains 4 latent fear scores (T1–T4) reflecting the true level of fear at time 1 (age 8–9 years), time 2 (age 13–14 years), time 3 (age 16–17 years), and time 4 (age 19–20 years). These latent variables are indexed by ratings of fear by parental report (P) (available for times 1–3) and by self-report (S) (available for times 2–4). The degree to which the parent- and self-reported fear ratings index the latent fear level is reflected by the paths λP and λS. The genetic and environmental influences on the latent fear scores are modeled as a Cholesky decomposition. See the text of this article for more details. F indicates the 4 genetic risk factors; f, the path from the genetic factors to the latent fear scores at each of the 4 ages; R, residual effects.
Figure 2
Figure 2
The proportion of total variance in fears accounted for by genetic factors through development. The y-axis represents the total phenotypic variance so the sum of all the factors equals the total heritability. Purple represents the first genetic factor starting at age 8 to 9 years. Blue represents the second genetic factor starting at age 13 to 14 years. Yellow represents the third genetic factor starting at age 16 to 17 years and green represents the fourth genetic factor acting only at age 19 to 20 years. A, Results for situational fears, the exact parameter estimates of which are seen in Table 5. B, Results for animal fears, the exact parameter estimates of which are seen in Table 6. C, Results for blood/injury fears, the exact parameter estimates of which are seen in Table 7.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Muris P, Merckelbach H. The etiology of childhood specific phobia: a multifactorial model. In: Vasey MW, Dadds MR, editors. The Developmental Psychopathology of Anxiety. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 355–385.
    1. Armfield JM. Cognitive vulnerability: a model of the etiology of fear. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26(6):746–768. - PubMed
    1. Silverman WK, Moreno J. Specific phobia. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2005;14(4):819–843. - PubMed
    1. Fyer AJ. Current approaches to etiology and pathophysiology of specific phobia. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44(12):1295–1304. - PubMed
    1. Marks IM. Fears, Phobias, and Rituals. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1987.

Publication types