Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Aug 7;275(1644):1727-35.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0302.

Encoding choosiness: female attraction requires prior physical contact with individual male scents in mice

Affiliations

Encoding choosiness: female attraction requires prior physical contact with individual male scents in mice

Steven A Ramm et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

Scents, detected through both the main and vomeronasal olfactory systems, play a crucial role in regulating reproductive behaviour in many mammals. In laboratory mice, female preference for airborne urinary scents from males (detected through the main olfactory system) is learnt through association with scents detected through the vomeronasal system during contact with the scent source. This may reflect a more complex assessment of individual males than that implied by laboratory mouse studies in which individual variation has largely been eliminated. To test this, we assessed female preference between male and female urine using wild house mice with natural individual genetic variation in urinary identity signals. We confirm that females exhibit a general preference for male over female urine when able to contact urine scents. However, they are only attracted to airborne urinary volatiles from individual males whose urine they have previously contacted. Even females with a natural exposure to many individuals of both sexes fail to develop generalized attraction to airborne male scents. This implies that information gained through contact with a specific male's scent is essential to stimulate attraction, providing a new perspective on the cues and olfactory pathways involved in sex recognition and mate assessment in rodents.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Preference for male (M) over female (F) urine when naive females can contact urine stimuli. Females were either (a) familiar with urine from the same individuals from pre-exposure prior to the test or (b) unfamiliar with the stimuli after pre-exposure to urine from different individuals. The response is broken down into time spent sniffing the stimulus, time under the stimulus not sniffing and total time under the stimulus urine (mean+s.e., n=16). The p-values indicate Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. The log ratio of total time under the male/female stimulus did not differ between familiar and unfamiliar tests (matched-pair t-test, t15=−0.89, p=0.39).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Preference for airborne male (M) over female (F) urinary odours according to prior exposure to urine stimuli. Prior to the preference test, naive females had pre-exposure to either airborne urinary odours (a,b) or contact with urine stimuli (c,d), where pre-exposure and test stimuli were either from the same individuals ((a,c) familiar) or from different individuals ((b,d) unfamiliar). The p-values indicate Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. Females preferred male over female airborne odours only in test (c) when they had contact with urine from the same individuals prior to the test (interaction term for log ratio in total time: F1,30=4.16, p=0.050). The preference in test (c) differed strongly from that following contact with urine from different individual donors ((c) versus (d), matched-pair t-test of log ratio in total time, t15=3.20, p=0.006), and more weakly from that following pre-exposure to airborne scent from the same individual donors ((c) versus (a), t-test: t30=1.78, p=0.085).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Preference of socially experienced females for airborne male (M) over female (F) urinary odours. Experienced females were either (a) familiar with soiled bedding from the same individual male prior to the test or (b) unfamiliar after pre-exposure to bedding from a different individual male. The p-values indicate Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. The log ratio of total time under the male/female stimulus differed between familiar and unfamiliar tests (matched-pair t-test, t9=3.08, p=0.013).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alder S. Origin of the golden hamster (Cricetus auratus) as a laboratory animal. Nature. 1948;162:256–257. doi:10.1038/162256b0 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Andersson M. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 1994. Sexual selection.
    1. Andersson M, Simmons L.W. Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006;21:296–302. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.015 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Beck J.A, Lloyd S, Hafezparast M, Lennon-Pierce M, Eppig J.T, Festing M.F, Fisher E.M. Genealogies of mouse inbred strains. Nat. Genet. 2000;24:23–25. doi:10.1038/71641 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Boyse E.A, Beauchamp G.K, Yamazaki K. The genetics of body scent. Trends Genet. 1987;3:97–102. doi:10.1016/0168-9525(87)90192-2 - DOI

Publication types