Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Aug;69(8):1223-7.
doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n0804.

Language in Alzheimer's disease

Affiliations

Language in Alzheimer's disease

Myron F Weiner et al. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008 Aug.

Abstract

Objective: To ascertain the clinical utility of language examination by psychiatrists in evaluating Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients.

Method: Data collected between 1986 and 2003 from a standardized psychiatric examination and neuropsychological testing of probable AD patients (diagnosed according to the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association) were gathered from the database of the University of Texas Southwestern Alzheimer's Disease Center, Dallas. The variables studied were articulation, word-finding ability, hypofluency, hyperfluency, repetition, confrontational naming, and semantic (category) fluency. Articulation, word-finding ability, hypofluency, hyperfluency, repetition, and confrontational naming were rated as normal or abnormal. Semantic fluency was scored numerically as the number of animals named in a minute. Cognitive impairment was assessed with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and global impairment by the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale.

Results: There was a significant association (p < .0001) between MMSE and CDR scores for all language measures except hyperfluency. The MMSE scores were higher in the group with responses rated as normal compared to those with abnormal responses. Patients with greater cognitive and global impairment named fewer animals in a minute.

Conclusions: Abnormal articulation and repetition of words were unusual and therefore would not be useful for early detection, but when present, were associated with more advanced disease. Impairment in fluency, animal naming, and confrontational naming were common and increased in frequency with greater cognitive and global impairment. Because animal naming is a numerical measure, changes in the number of animals named in a minute can be used to monitor disease progression.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have no other personal affiliations or financial relationships with any commercial interest to disclose relative to the article.

References

    1. Forbes-McKay KE, Venneri A. Detecting subtle spontaneous language decline in early Alzheimer's disease with a picture description task. Neurol Sci. 2005;26:243–254. - PubMed
    1. Murdoch BE, Chenery HJ, Wilks V, et al. Language disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain Lang. 1987;31:122–137. - PubMed
    1. Marczinski CA, Kertesz A. Category and letter fluency in semantic dementia, primary progressive aphasia, and Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2006;97:258–265. - PubMed
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. - PubMed
    1. Binetti G, Magni E, Cappa SF, et al. Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease: an analysis of category fluency. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995;17:82–89. - PubMed

Publication types