Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Nov 22;275(1651):2603-7.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0754.

Learning by embryos and the ghost of predation future

Affiliations

Learning by embryos and the ghost of predation future

Alicia Mathis et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

Most research on the effects of exposure to stressful stimuli during embryonic development has focused on post-embryonic behaviour that appears to be abnormal or maladaptive. Here, we tested whether exposure to some stressful stimuli (predatory cues) can lead to post-embryonic behaviour that is adaptive. When eggs of ringed salamanders (Ambystoma annulatum) were exposed to chemical cues from predators, post-hatching larvae showed reduced activity and greater shelter-seeking behaviour; larvae that had been exposed to control cues did not show these behaviours. In addition, wood frog (Rana sylvatica)tadpoles learned to respond to chemical cues from unfamiliar predators with danger based on embryonic conditioning. Therefore, if embryonic experience is a good predictor of future risk, learning associated with exposure to negative stimuli during development may be adaptive.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Post-hatching behaviour of Ambystoma annulatum larvae that had been exposed as embryos to chemical stimuli from predators (cannibalistic conspecifics), non-predators (tadpoles of R. catesbeiana) or to a blank control. (a) Activity measured as total number of lines crossed in 5 min (mean±1 s.e.). Statistical analysis was an ANOVA followed by Tukey's post hoc comparisons; different letters indicate significant differences. (b) Habitat choice measured as proportion of larvae found in the plants after 5 hours. Statistical analysis was the Fisher's exact probability test; *p=0.011; n.s., p>0.15.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Activity measured as proportional change in line crosses (mean±1 s.e.) for wood frog tadpoles that had been exposed to tadpole alarm cues paired with newt odour (TP+NO), well water paired with newt odour (WW+NO) or well water only (WW) in the egg. Different shading patterns represent the responses of tadpoles from different clutches.

References

    1. Brodman R, Jaskula J. Activity and microhabitat use during interactions among five species of pond-breeding salamander larvae. Herpetologica. 2002;58:346–354. doi:10.1655/0018-0831(2002)058[0346:AAMUDI]2.0.CO;2 - DOI
    1. Brown G.E. Learning about danger: chemical alarm cues and local risk assessment in prey fishes. Fish Fish. 2003;4:227–234. doi:10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00132.x - DOI
    1. Davis P.D, Glynn L.M, Schetter C.D, Hobel C, Chicz-Demet A, Sandman C.A. Corticotropin-releasing hormone during pregnancy is associated with infant temperament. Devel. Neurosci. 2005;27:299–305. doi:10.1159/000086709 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Dugovic C, Maccari S, Weibel L, Turek F.W, Van Reeth O. High corticosterone levels in prenatally stressed rats predict persistent paradoxical sleep alterations. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:8656–8664. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Gosner K.L. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica. 1960;16:183–190.

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources