Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Sep;44(5):1277-87.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.5.1277.

Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity

Affiliations

Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity

Raedy M Ping et al. Dev Psychol. 2008 Sep.

Abstract

Including gesture in instruction facilitates learning. Why? One possibility is that gesture points out objects in the immediate context and thus helps ground the words learners hear in the world they see. Previous work on gesture's role in instruction has used gestures that either point to or trace paths on objects, thus providing support for this hypothesis. The experiments described here investigated the possibility that gesture helps children learn even when it is not produced in relation to an object but is instead produced "in the air." Children were given instruction in Piagetian conservation problems with or without gesture and with or without concrete objects. The results indicate that children given instruction with speech and gesture learned more about conservation than children given instruction with speech alone, whether or not objects were present during instruction. Gesture in instruction can thus help learners learn even when those gestures do not direct attention to visible objects, suggesting that gesture can do more for learners than simply ground arbitrary, symbolic language in the physical, observable world.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Stills from a video illustrating gesture plus speech instruction in the objects present (top) and objects absent (bottom) conditions. In the middle is the speech that accompanied the gestures; speech was identical in all conditions.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Number of correct answers (left graph) and correct explanations (right graph) children added to their repertoires on the posttest in Experiment 1. Responses are categorized according to whether children received instruction in gesture and speech or speech alone, and according to whether the task objects were visible during instruction.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Proportion of children who added the explanation taught by the experimenter (left graph) and who added correct explanations not taught by the experimenter (right graph) to their spoken repertoires after instruction in Experiment 1. Responses are categorized according to whether the children received instruction in gesture and speech or speech alone, and according to whether the task objects were visible during instruction.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Number of correct answers (left graph) and correct explanations (right graph) children added to their repertoires on the posttest in Experiment 2. Responses are categorized according to whether children received instruction in gesture and speech or speech alone, and according to whether the task objects were visible during instruction
Figure 5
Figure 5
Proportion of children who added the explanation taught by the experimenter (left graph) and who added correct explanations not taught by the experimenter (right graph) to their spoken repertoires after instruction in Experiment 2. Responses are categorized according to whether the children received instruction in gesture and speech or speech alone, and according to whether the task objects were visible during instruction.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Proportion of children who added the explanation taught by the experimenter (left graph) and who added correct explanations not taught by the experimenter (right graph) to their spoken repertoires after instruction in Experiment 2, when children are matched for their performance (number of correct explanations) at pretest (N = 9 per condition). Responses are categorized according to whether the children received instruction in gesture and speech or speech alone, and according to whether the task objects were visible during instruction.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alibali MW, Goldin-Meadow S. Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child’s state of mind. Cognitive Psychology. 1993;25(4):468–523. - PubMed
    1. Broaders S, Cook SW, Mitchell Z, Goldin-Meadow S. Making children gesture reveals implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2007 in press. - PubMed
    1. Church RB, Ayman-Nolley S, Mahootian S. The role of gesture in bilingual education: Does gesture enhance learning? International Journal of Bilingual Education & Bilingualism. 2004;7(4):303–320.
    1. Church RB, Goldin-Meadow S. The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition. 1986;23(1):43–71. - PubMed
    1. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd Edition Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988.

Publication types