Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Feb 23;5(1):125-9.
doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0469.

Why are there so few smart mammals (but so many smart birds)?

Affiliations

Why are there so few smart mammals (but so many smart birds)?

Karin Isler et al. Biol Lett. .

Abstract

The expensive brain hypothesis predicts an interspecific link between relative brain size and life-history pace. Indeed, animals with relatively large brains have reduced rates of growth and reproduction. However, they also have increased total lifespan. Here we show that the reduction in production with increasing brain size is not fully compensated by the increase in lifespan. Consequently, the maximum rate of population increase (rmax) is negatively correlated with brain mass. This result is not due to a confounding effect of body size, indicating that the well-known correlation between rmax and body size is driven by brain size, at least among homeothermic vertebrates. Thus, each lineage faces a 'grey ceiling', i.e. a maximum viable brain size, beyond which rmax is so low that the risk of local or species extinction is very high. We found that the steep decline in rmax with brain size is absent in taxa with allomaternal offspring provisioning, such as cooperatively breeding mammals and most altricial birds. These taxa thus do not face a lineage-specific grey ceiling, which explains the far greater number of independent origins of large brain size in birds than mammals. We also predict that (absolute and relative) brain size is an important predictor of macroevolutionary extinction patterns.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
(a) Least-squares regression of ln rmax on ln brain mass in eutherian mammals (n=536, Homo excluded). The regressions of the residuals of ln rmax on the residuals of ln brain mass are shown for (b) altricials and (c) precocials separately. Full statistical details are given in the electronic supplementary material.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(a) Least-squares regression of ln rmax on ln brain mass in birds (n=389). The regressions of the residuals of ln rmax on the residuals of ln brain mass are shown for (b) altricials and (c) precocials separately. Full statistical details are given in the electronic supplementary material.

References

    1. Aiello L.C., Wheeler P. The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive-system in human and primate evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 1995;36:199–221. doi:10.1086/204350 - DOI
    1. Barrickman N.L., Bastian M.L., Isler K., Van Schaik C.P. Life history costs and benefits of encephalization: a comparative test using data from long-term studies of primates in the wild. J. Hum. Evol. 2008;54:568–590. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.08.012 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brook B.W., Bowman D. One equation fits overkill: why allometry underpins both prehistoric and modern body size-biased extinctions. Popul. Ecol. 2005;47:137–141. doi:10.1007/s10144-005-0213-4 - DOI
    1. Cockburn A. Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2006;273:1375–1383. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3458 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cole L. The population consequences of life-history phenomena. Q. Rev. Biol. 1954;29:103–137. doi:10.1086/400074 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources