Win-shift, lose-stay: contingent switching and contextual interference in motor learning
- PMID: 19093603
- DOI: 10.2466/pms.107.2.407-418
Win-shift, lose-stay: contingent switching and contextual interference in motor learning
Abstract
Learners (n = 48) practiced three multisegment movements with distinct target movement times. Four practice groups were compared: blocked, random, and two groups who had a win-shift/lose-stay schedule (WSLS1 and WSLS2). For these latter groups switching between practice tasks was performance-contingent: within 5% of target time for 1 or 2 consecutive trials, respectively. During acquisition, blocked performance was more accurate than for both random and WSLS2 groups. The WSLS1 group performed between blocked and random groups, but did not differ from either. In a next-day retention test, the random group scored better than the blocked group. The WSLS1 group performed similarly to the random practice while the WSLS2 group's scores were similar to those of the blocked group. Results encourage further study of similar practice schedules.
Similar articles
-
Contextual interference effects with two tasks.Percept Mot Skills. 2007 Aug;105(1):177-83. doi: 10.2466/pms.105.1.177-183. Percept Mot Skills. 2007. PMID: 17918561
-
Effects of contextual interference and age on acquisition, retention, and transfer of motor skill.Percept Mot Skills. 1999 Apr;88(2):437-47. doi: 10.2466/pms.1999.88.2.437. Percept Mot Skills. 1999. PMID: 10483636
-
Contextual interference and augmented feedback: is there an additive effect for motor learning?Hum Mov Sci. 2011 Dec;30(6):1092-101. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.02.004. Epub 2011 Apr 27. Hum Mov Sci. 2011. PMID: 21524808
-
Practice schedules for surgical skills: the role of task characteristics and proactive interference on psychomotor skills acquisition.J Surg Educ. 2013 Nov-Dec;70(6):789-95. doi: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2013.06.014. Epub 2013 Aug 6. J Surg Educ. 2013. PMID: 24209657 Review.
-
Unifying practice schedules in the timescales of motor learning and performance.Hum Mov Sci. 2018 Jun;59:153-169. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.04.004. Epub 2018 Apr 21. Hum Mov Sci. 2018. PMID: 29684760 Review.
Cited by
-
High contextual interference improves retention in motor learning: systematic review and meta-analysis.Sci Rep. 2024 Jul 10;14(1):15974. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-65753-3. Sci Rep. 2024. PMID: 38987617 Free PMC article.
-
Interactive curriculum learning increases and homogenizes motor smoothness.Sci Rep. 2024 Feb 3;14(1):2843. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-53253-3. Sci Rep. 2024. PMID: 38310201 Free PMC article.
-
Quantifying Contextual Interference and Its Effect on Skill Transfer in Skilled Youth Tennis Players.Front Psychol. 2017 Nov 3;8:1931. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01931. eCollection 2017. Front Psychol. 2017. PMID: 29163306 Free PMC article.
-
Neural Mechanisms of the Contextual Interference Effect and Parameter Similarity on Motor Learning in Older Adults: An EEG Study.Front Aging Neurosci. 2020 Jun 12;12:173. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00173. eCollection 2020. Front Aging Neurosci. 2020. PMID: 32595488 Free PMC article.
-
Absence of equifinality of hand position in a double-step unloading task.Exp Brain Res. 2010 Aug;205(2):167-82. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2350-z. Epub 2010 Jul 10. Exp Brain Res. 2010. PMID: 20623112
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Miscellaneous