Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Feb;32(2):73-8.
doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.10.004. Epub 2009 Jan 10.

Temporal maps and informativeness in associative learning

Affiliations

Temporal maps and informativeness in associative learning

Peter D Balsam et al. Trends Neurosci. 2009 Feb.

Erratum in

  • Trends Neurosci. 2009 Mar;32(3):127

Abstract

Neurobiological research on learning assumes that temporal contiguity is essential for association formation, but what constitutes temporal contiguity has never been specified. We review evidence that learning depends, instead, on learning a temporal map. Temporal relations between events are encoded even from single experiences. The speed with which an anticipatory response emerges is proportional to the informativeness of the encoded relation between a predictive stimulus or event and the event it predicts. This principle yields a quantitative account of the heretofore undefined, but theoretically crucial, concept of temporal pairing, an account in quantitative accord with surprising experimental findings. The same principle explains the basic results in the cue competition literature, which motivated the Rescorla-Wagner model and most other contemporary models of associative learning. The essential feature of a memory mechanism in this account is its ability to encode quantitative information.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure I
Figure I
Learning protocols and the behavioral results they produce (a CR to the CS, or not). Basic : the US occurs only during the CS but at unpredictable times within it: leads to a CR. Delay: the US occurs only at the termination of a CS of fixed duration, ICS: leads to a CR. Inhibitory: the US occurs only when CS is absent (ergo, CS never paired with US): leads to a CR (such as CS avoidance if the US is appetitive or CS approach if the US is aversive). The remaining protocols are cue-competition protocols. Truly random: the times of US occurrence are completely independent; hence, they sometimes occur during CS ’by coincidence’: a CR to the CS does not develop but a strong CR does develop to the experimental chamber (the spatial context), which is a competing cue. Blocking: trained during Phase 1 with CS1 and then during Phase 2 with CS2 and CS1, presented ’in compound’ (together): a CR to CS2 does not develop. Overshadowing: CS1 and CS2 presented in compound from the outset; a CR develops to one or the other CS but not both (Box 4). Relative validity: (1) CS2 is always presented in compound; half the time it is compounded with CS1 and half the time with CS3. The US occurs only when it is compounded with CS1. A CR develops only to CS1, despite fact that US is paired with CS2 on half of all CS2 occurrences. (2) Same as in RV–1 except that now pairing of US with CS2 occurs on the half of the trials when it appears in compound with CS1 and half the trials when it appears in compound with CS3. CR develops only to CS2, despite fact that the US is paired with the other two CSs just as frequently (on half of all their occurrences) and with CS2 no more frequently than in RV–1.
Figure I
Figure I
(a) Trials to acquisition (a measure of associability) plotted as a function of the CS–US interval, either when the average interval between USs, ĪB, is fixed (solid line) or when the ratio of this interval to CS duration, ĪB/ICS, is fixed (dashed line). (b) Trials to acquisition versus ĪB/ICS on double logarithmic coordinates. The solid line is the best fitting linear regression; the dashed curves are the 95% confidence interval. Both plots adapted, with permission, from Ref. [5].
Figure 1
Figure 1
CR timing in goldfish as Pavlovian training progresses. A 5 s visual CS terminated in mild shock (US). Training trials were intermixed with probe trials, during which the CS remained on for 45 s with no US. Movement as a function of time in the CS is shown for blocks of 5 sessions (50 CS–US pairings) during the long unreinforced trials. The amount of anticipatory activity increased as training progressed, but the timing of the CR was appropriate even in the earliest part of training. Modified, with permission, from Ref. [18].

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Rieke F, et al. Spikes: Exploring the neural code. MIT Press; 1997.
    1. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black AH, Prokasy WF, editors. Classical Conditioning II. Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1972. pp. 64–99.
    1. Mackintosh NJ. Conditioning and Associative Learning. Oxford University Press; 1983.
    1. Hawkins RD, Kandel ER. Is there a cell-biological alphabet for simple forms of learning? Psychol. Rev. 1984;91:375–391. - PubMed
    1. Gallistel CR, Gibbon J. Time, rate, and conditioning. Psychol. Rev. 2000;107:289–344. - PubMed