Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Mar;125(3):1658-65.
doi: 10.1121/1.3068441.

Low-frequency speech cues and simulated electric-acoustic hearing

Affiliations

Low-frequency speech cues and simulated electric-acoustic hearing

Christopher A Brown et al. J Acoust Soc Am. 2009 Mar.

Abstract

The addition of low-frequency acoustic information to real or simulated electric stimulation (so-called electric-acoustic stimulation or EAS) often results in large improvements in intelligibility, particularly in competing backgrounds. This may reflect the availability of fundamental frequency (F0) information in the acoustic region. The contributions of F0 and the amplitude envelope (as well as voicing) of speech to simulated EAS was examined by replacing the low-frequency speech with a tone that was modulated in frequency to track the F0 of the speech, in amplitude with the envelope of the low-frequency speech, or both. A four-channel vocoder simulated electric hearing. Significant benefit over vocoder alone was observed with the addition of a tone carrying F0 or envelope cues, and both cues combined typically provided significantly more benefit than either alone. The intelligibility improvement over vocoder was between 24 and 57 percentage points, and was unaffected by the presence of a tone carrying these cues from a background talker. These results confirm the importance of the F0 of target speech for EAS (in simulation). They indicate that significant benefit can be provided by a tone carrying F0 and amplitude envelope cues. The results support a glimpsing account of EAS and argue against segregation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean percent correct scores, ±1 standard error. Each curve represents a different background, which was present in the vocoder region only. Processing conditions are along the x axis. The output of the vocoder was presented alone (V) or with different low-frequency cues, which included 500 Hz low-pass speech (V∕500), a tone with only voicing applied (VT), a tone modulated in frequency by the fundamental of the target talker (VTF0), a tone modulated in amplitude by the envelope of the low-pass speech (VTenv), and a tone modulated in both frequency and amplitude (VTF0-env). Mean intelligibility provided by the low-frequency cues themselves is depicted by diamonds. Values to the right of the VTF0-env data points are percentage points of improvement over vocoder alone. Although the processing variable, which is depicted along the x axis, is not a continuous variable, the different levels of processing within each background are connected with lines for clarity.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean percent correct scores, ±1 standard error. Squares and circles indicate performance when the background was a male and a female, respectively. Unfilled and filled symbols indicate performance when the low-frequency cue was speech and a modulated tone, respectively. Different low-frequency stimulus conditions are represented along the x axis, and are no low-frequency stimulus (none), target only (target), background only (background), or target and background (target+background). Although the low-frequency stimulus variable, which is depicted along the x axis, is not a continuous variable, the different levels of this variable within each background gender∕low-frequency cue combination are connected with lines for clarity.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. ANSI (1996). “ANSI S3.6-1996, Specifications for audiometers,” (American National Standards Institute, New York).
    1. Assmann, P. F. (1999). “Fundamental frequency and the intelligibility of competing voices,” Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic ScienceSan Francisco, August.
    1. Assmann, P. F., and Summerfield, Q. (1990). “Modeling the perception of concurrent vowels: Vowels with different fundamental frequencies,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. JASMAN10.1121/1.399772 88, 680–697. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Auditec (1997). “Auditory Tests (Revised),” Compact Disc, Auditec, St. Louis.
    1. Bell, T. S., Dirks, D. D., and Trine, T. D. (1992). “Frequency-importance functions for words in high- and low-context sentences,” J. Speech Hear. Res. JSPHAH 35, 950–959. - PubMed

Publication types