Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Feb;24(2):187-201.
doi: 10.1177/0269881109103203. Epub 2009 Mar 20.

MDMA (ecstasy) use is associated with reduced BOLD signal change during semantic recognition in abstinent human polydrug users: a preliminary fMRI study

Affiliations

MDMA (ecstasy) use is associated with reduced BOLD signal change during semantic recognition in abstinent human polydrug users: a preliminary fMRI study

V Raj et al. J Psychopharmacol. 2010 Feb.

Abstract

3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) users have impaired verbal memory, and voxel-based morphometry has shown decreased grey matter in Brodmann area (BA) 18, 21 and 45. Because these regions play a role in verbal memory, we hypothesized that MDMA users would show altered brain activation in these areas during performance of a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task that probed semantic verbal memory. Polysubstance users enriched for MDMA exposure participated in a semantic memory encoding and recognition fMRI task that activated left BA 9, 18, 21/22 and 45. Primary outcomes were percent blood oxygen level-dependent signal change in left BA 9, 18, 21/22 and 45, accuracy and response time. During semantic recognition, lifetime MDMA use was associated with decreased activation in left BA 9, 18 and 21/22 but not 45. This was partly influenced by contributions from cannabis and cocaine use. MDMA exposure was not associated with accuracy or response time during the semantic recognition task. During semantic recognition, MDMA exposure was associated with reduced regional brain activation in regions mediating verbal memory. These findings partially overlap with previous structural evidence for reduced grey matter in MDMA users and may, in part, explain the consistent verbal memory impairments observed in other studies of MDMA users.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Semantic Task
Subjects encoded a block of 5 words or pseudo-words followed by a recall task using novel homophones as distracter images. Subjects were instructed to press a button corresponding to right or left screen location of previously encoded items. Semantic encode: Activation during semantic encoding was defined as (word encode minus pseudo-word encode). Semantic recognition: Activation during semantic recognition was defined as (word recognition minus pseudo-word recognition).
Figure 2
Figure 2. Activation to Word Encode and Word Recognition
Task-induced regional brain activation maps for word encode and word recognition. Maps were thresholded at a false discovery rate (FDR) of < 0.05 (see methods for details). Rows show (top to bottom) regional brain activation to Brodmann Area (BA) 9, 18, 21/22 and 45. White arrowheads point to BA in each brain view. For each condition (word encode or word recognition) activation maps are overlayed on a group average structural brain in (left to right) coronal, sagittal, and axial sections. Right side of figure is left side of brain for coronal and axial sections. Right side is posterior for sagittal sections. Scale bar indicates t values ranging from −8.00 to plus 8.00. Warm colors are positive t, indicating increased activation with task activity. Cool colors are negative t, indicating decreased activation with task activity. All regions of interest used for analysis showed increased task-induced activation.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Regional Semantic percent BOLD signal change
Bar chart indicating regional semantic percent BOLD signal change as the percent signal change in a region of interest for word encode or recognition minus pseudo-word encode or recognition.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allen PP, Cleare AJ, Lee F, Fusar-Poli P, Tunstall N, Fu CH, Brammer MJ, McGuire PK. Effect of acute tryptophan depletion on pre-frontal engagement. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;187:486–497. - PubMed
    1. Amunts K, Weiss PH, Mohlberg H, Pieperhoff P, Eickhoff S, Gurd JM, Marshall JC, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Zilles K. Analysis of neural mechanisms underlying verbal fluency in cytoarchitectonically defined stereotaxic space--the roles of Brodmann areas 44 and 45. Neuroimage. 2004;22:42–56. - PubMed
    1. Ardila A, Rosselli M, Strumwasser S. Neuropsychological deficits in chronic cocaine abusers. Int.J Neurosci. 1991;57:73–79. - PubMed
    1. Babiloni C, Ferretti A, Del GC, Carducci F, Vecchio F, Romani GL, Rossini PM. Human cortical responses during one-bit delayed-response tasks: an fMRI study. Brain Res.Bull. 2005;65:383–390. - PubMed
    1. Back-Madruga C, Boone KB, Chang L, Grob CS, Lee A, Nations H, Poland RE. Neuropsychological effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or ecstasy) in recreational users. Clin Neuropsychol. 2003;17:446–459. - PubMed

Publication types