Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others
- PMID: 19338505
- DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04425.x
Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others
Abstract
Understanding the goals or intentions of other people requires a broad range of evaluative processes including the decoding of biological motion, knowing about object properties, and abilities for recognizing task space requirements and social contexts. It is becoming increasingly evident that some of this decoding is based in part on the simulation of other people's behavior within our own nervous system. This review focuses on aspects of action understanding that rely on embodied cognition, that is, the knowledge of the body and how it interacts with the world. This form of cognition provides an essential knowledge base from which action simulation can be used to decode at least some actions performed by others. Recent functional imaging studies or action understanding are interpreted with a goal of defining conditions when simulation operations occur and how this relates with other constructs, including top-down versus bottom-up processing and the functional distinctions between action observation and social networks. From this it is argued that action understanding emerges from the engagement of highly flexible computational hierarchies driven by simulation, object properties, social context, and kinematic constraints and where the hierarchy is driven by task structure rather than functional or strict anatomic rules.
Similar articles
-
Captured by motion: dance, action understanding, and social cognition.Brain Cogn. 2011 Nov;77(2):231-6. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.08.005. Epub 2011 Aug 30. Brain Cogn. 2011. PMID: 21880410 Review.
-
The shared circuits model (SCM): how control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading.Behav Brain Sci. 2008 Feb;31(1):1-22; discussion 22-58. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07003123. Behav Brain Sci. 2008. PMID: 18394222
-
Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis.Neuroimage. 2009 Nov 15;48(3):564-84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009. Epub 2009 Jun 11. Neuroimage. 2009. PMID: 19524046 Review.
-
Action, the foundation for cognitive development.Scand J Psychol. 2009 Dec;50(6):617-23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00780.x. Scand J Psychol. 2009. PMID: 19930261 Review.
-
The motor theory of social cognition: a critique.Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Jan;9(1):21-5. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005. PMID: 15639437 Review.
Cited by
-
Common neural correlates of emotion perception in humans.Hum Brain Mapp. 2015 Oct;36(10):4184-201. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22910. Epub 2015 Jul 28. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015. PMID: 26219630 Free PMC article.
-
Graspable objects shape number processing.Front Hum Neurosci. 2011 Dec 6;5:147. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00147. eCollection 2011. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011. PMID: 22164141 Free PMC article.
-
Passive motion paradigm: an alternative to optimal control.Front Neurorobot. 2011 Dec 27;5:4. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2011.00004. eCollection 2011. Front Neurorobot. 2011. PMID: 22207846 Free PMC article.
-
Seeing Through Each Other's Hearts: Inferring Others' Heart Rate as a Function of Own Heart Rate Perception and Perceived Social Intelligence.Affect Sci. 2022 Nov 2;3(4):862-877. doi: 10.1007/s42761-022-00151-4. eCollection 2022 Dec. Affect Sci. 2022. PMID: 36519151 Free PMC article.
-
The Good, the bad, and the just: justice sensitivity predicts neural response during moral evaluation of actions performed by others.J Neurosci. 2014 Mar 19;34(12):4161-6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4648-13.2014. J Neurosci. 2014. PMID: 24647937 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Medical