Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2009 Jul;73(4):463-76.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0230-6. Epub 2009 Apr 7.

Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: a case for animal-human translational models

Affiliations
Free PMC article
Review

Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: a case for animal-human translational models

Sanne de Wit et al. Psychol Res. 2009 Jul.
Free PMC article

Abstract

Associative accounts of goal-directed action, developed in the fields of human ideomotor action and that of animal learning, can capture cognitive belief-desire psychology of human decision-making. Whereas outcome-response accounts can account for the fact that the thought of a goal can call to mind the action that has previously procured this goal, response-outcome accounts capture decision-making processes that start out with the consideration of possible response alternatives followed only in the second instance by evaluation of their consequences. We argue that while the outcome-response mechanism plays a crucial role in response priming effects, the response-outcome mechanism is particularly important for action selection on the basis of current needs and desires. We therefore develop an integrative account that encapsulates these two routes of action selection within the framework of the associative-cybernetic model. This model has the additional benefit of providing mechanisms for the incentive modulation of goal-directed action and for the development of behavioural autonomy, and therefore provides a promising account of the multi-faceted process of animal as well as human instrumental decision-making.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Schematic representation of the basic architecture of the associative-cybernetic model

References

    1. None
    2. Adams, C. D. (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,34B, 77–98.
    1. None
    2. Adams, C. D., & Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,33B, 109–121.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.1036', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.111.4.1036'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '15482072', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15482072/'}]}
    2. Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglas, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review,111, 1036–1060. - PubMed
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00062-2', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(01)00062-2'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11881904', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11881904/'}]}
    2. Ansorge, U. (2002). Spatial intention-response compatibility. Acta Psychologia,109(3), 285–299. - PubMed
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '4838073', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/4838073/'}]}
    2. Asratyan, E. A. (1974). Conditional reflex theory and motivated behavior. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis,34, 15–31. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources