Comparing black-capped (Poecile atricapillus) and mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli): use of geometric and featural information in a spatial orientation task
- PMID: 19381699
- DOI: 10.1007/s10071-009-0222-3
Comparing black-capped (Poecile atricapillus) and mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli): use of geometric and featural information in a spatial orientation task
Abstract
Since Cheng (Cognition 23:149-178, 1986) first proposed the "geometric module" in rats, a great deal of research has focused on how other species use geometric information and how geometric encoding may differ across species. Here, hand-reared and wild-caught black-capped chickadees and wild-caught mountain chickadees searched for food hidden in one corner in a rectangular environment. Previous research has shown that mountain chickadees do not spontaneously encode geometric information when a salient feature is present near the goal location. Using a slightly different training and testing procedure, we found that both hand-reared and wild-caught black-capped chickadees encoded geometric information, even in the presence of a salient landmark. Some, but not all, mountain chickadees also encoded geometric information. Overall, our results suggest that use of geometric information may be a less preferred strategy for mountain chickadees than for either wild-caught or hand-reared black-capped chickadees. To our knowledge, this is the first direct interspecies comparison of use of geometric information in a spatial orientation task.
Similar articles
-
Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry.Biol Lett. 2005 Sep 22;1(3):314-7. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0347. Biol Lett. 2005. PMID: 17148196 Free PMC article.
-
Categorization and discrimination of "chick-a-dee" calls by wild-caught and hand-reared chickadees.Behav Processes. 2008 Feb;77(2):166-76. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2007.08.003. Epub 2007 Aug 31. Behav Processes. 2008. PMID: 17928165
-
Frequency-range discriminations and absolute pitch in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus), mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli), and zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata).J Comp Psychol. 2006 Aug;120(3):217-28. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.217. J Comp Psychol. 2006. PMID: 16893259
-
Doing Socrates experiment right: controlled rearing studies of geometrical knowledge in animals.Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009 Feb;19(1):20-6. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.002. Epub 2009 Mar 18. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009. PMID: 19299120 Review.
-
Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric module.Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Sep;12(9):355-61. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.06.004. Epub 2008 Aug 4. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008. PMID: 18684662 Review.
Cited by
-
What scatter-hoarding animals have taught us about small-scale navigation.Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010 Mar 27;365(1542):901-14. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0214. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010. PMID: 20156815 Free PMC article. Review.
-
25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective.Psychon Bull Rev. 2013 Dec;20(6):1033-54. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0416-1. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013. PMID: 23456412 Review.
-
Hand rearing affects emotional responses but not basic cognitive performance in European starlings.Anim Behav. 2013 Jul;86(1):127-138. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.002. Anim Behav. 2013. PMID: 23888084 Free PMC article.
-
Flower bats (Glossophaga soricina) and fruit bats (Carollia perspicillata) rely on spatial cues over shapes and scents when relocating food.PLoS One. 2010 May 25;5(5):e10808. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010808. PLoS One. 2010. PMID: 20520841 Free PMC article.
-
On the transfer of spatial learning between geometrically different shaped environments in the terrestrial toad, Rhinella arenarum.Anim Cogn. 2020 Jan;23(1):55-70. doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01315-9. Epub 2019 Oct 18. Anim Cogn. 2020. PMID: 31628550
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources