The ups and downs (and lefts and rights) of synaesthetic number forms: validation from spatial cueing and SNARC-type tasks
- PMID: 19660746
- DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.015
The ups and downs (and lefts and rights) of synaesthetic number forms: validation from spatial cueing and SNARC-type tasks
Abstract
Typically, numbers are spatially represented using a mental 'number line' running from left to right. Individuals with number-form synaesthesia experience numbers as occupying specific spatial coordinates that are much more complex than a typical number line. Two synaesthetes (L and B) describe experiencing the numbers 1 through 10 running vertically from bottom to top, 10-20 horizontally from left to right, 21-40 from right to left, etc. We investigated whether their number forms could bias their spatial attention using a cueing paradigm and a SNARC-type task. In both experiments, the synaesthetes' responses confirmed their synaesthetic number forms. When making odd-even judgments for the numbers 1, 2, 8, and 9, they showed SNARC-compatibility effects for up-down movements (aligned with their number form), but not left-right (misaligned) movements. We conceptually replicated these biases using a spatial cueing paradigm. Both synaesthetes showed significantly faster response times to detect targets on the bottom of the display if preceded by a low number (1, 2), and the top of the display if preceded by a high number (8, 9), whereas they showed no cueing effects when targets appeared on the left or right (misaligned with their number forms). They were however reliably faster to detect left targets following the presentation of numbers 10 and 11, and right targets following numbers 19 and 20 (since 10-20 runs from left to right). In sum, cueing and SNARC tasks can be used to empirically verify synaesthetic number forms, and show that numbers can direct spatial attention to these idiosyncratic locations.
Similar articles
-
What information is critical to elicit interference in number-form synaesthesia?Cortex. 2009 Nov-Dec;45(10):1200-16. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.06.011. Epub 2009 Jul 7. Cortex. 2009. PMID: 19698941
-
A different outlook on time: visual and auditory month names elicit different mental vantage points for a time-space synaesthete.Cortex. 2009 Nov-Dec;45(10):1217-28. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.014. Epub 2009 Jul 9. Cortex. 2009. PMID: 19665700
-
Spatial forms and mental imagery.Cortex. 2009 Nov-Dec;45(10):1229-45. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.06.013. Epub 2009 Jul 7. Cortex. 2009. PMID: 19665116
-
[Neuropsychological approach to visual attention].Brain Nerve. 2007 Jan;59(1):23-30. Brain Nerve. 2007. PMID: 17354375 Review. Japanese.
-
Automaticity in sequence-space synaesthesia: a critical appraisal of the evidence.Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1165-86. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.013. Epub 2012 Nov 6. Cortex. 2013. PMID: 23237480 Review.
Cited by
-
Effects of non-symbolic numerical information suggest the existence of magnitude-space synesthesia.Cogn Process. 2012 Aug;13 Suppl 1:S179-83. doi: 10.1007/s10339-012-0449-9. Cogn Process. 2012. PMID: 22810422
-
Mental Number Representations in 2D Space.Front Psychol. 2019 Feb 5;10:172. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00172. eCollection 2019. Front Psychol. 2019. PMID: 30804847 Free PMC article.
-
Attentional cueing in numerical cognition.Front Psychol. 2014 Dec 1;5:1381. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01381. eCollection 2014. Front Psychol. 2014. PMID: 25520689 Free PMC article. No abstract available.
-
What spatial coordinate defines color-space synesthesia?Brain Cogn. 2016 Jun;105:88-94. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.04.002. Epub 2016 Apr 21. Brain Cogn. 2016. PMID: 27107251 Free PMC article.
-
Implications of number-space synesthesia on the automaticity of numerical processing.Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1352-62. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.019. Epub 2012 Apr 10. Cortex. 2013. PMID: 22578710 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources