Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Oct;111(1):8-19.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.07.009. Epub 2009 Aug 21.

A common mechanism in verb and noun naming deficits in Alzheimer's patients

Affiliations

A common mechanism in verb and noun naming deficits in Alzheimer's patients

Amit Almor et al. Brain Lang. 2009 Oct.

Abstract

We tested the ability of Alzheimer's patients and elderly controls to name living and non-living nouns, and manner and instrument verbs. Patients' error patterns and relative performance with different categories showed evidence of graceful degradation for both nouns and verbs, with particular domain-specific impairments for living nouns and instrument verbs. Our results support feature-based, semantic representations for nouns and verbs and support the role of inter-correlated features in noun impairment, and the role of noun knowledge in instrument verb impairment.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Noun and verb naming accuracy (a) and naming latency (b) for both groups. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.
Figure 2
Figure 2
The relation between patients’ age and noun-verb accuracy impairment difference scores.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Naming accuracy and correct response latencies by category for nouns and verbs. Categories are sorted from right to left by semantic domain (living vs. non-living nouns and instrument vs. manner verbs) and by AD patients accuracy within each domain. A line graph is used to highlight the similarity in the overall pattern of the two groups and not as indicating an underlying continuous scale. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Distribution of semantic errors for nouns and verbs for the EN and AD groups and for high and low performing AD subgroups. Error rates are shown as percentages from the total number of semantic errors (contrast coordinate, superordinate, cross category, and don’t know). Error bars show the standard error of the mean.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Naming accuracy for living and non-living nouns (a) and manner and instrument verbs (b). Error bars show the standard error of the mean.
Figure 6
Figure 6
The relation between AD patients’ noun-verb and living-non-living accuracy impairment difference scores.
Figure 7
Figure 7
The relation between patients’ noun accuracy and their (a) living-non-living impairment difference scores and (b) manner-instrument impairment difference scores.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aronoff JM, Gonnerman LM, Almor A, Arunachalam S, Kempler D, Andersen ES. Information content versus relational knowledge: Semantic deficits in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(1):21–35. - PubMed
    1. Bird H, Howard D, Franklin S. Why is a verb like an inanimate object? Grammatical category and semantic category deficits. Brain and Language. 2000;72(3):246–309. - PubMed
    1. Boland JE, Tanenhaus MK, Garnsey SM. Evidence for the immediate use of verb control information in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language. 1990;29:413–432.
    1. Bonilla JL, Johnson MK. Semantic space in Alzheimer's disease patients. Neuropsychology. 1995;9(3):345–353.
    1. Bowles NL, Obler LK, Albert ML. Naming errors in healthy aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Cortex. 1987;23(3):519–524. - PubMed

Publication types