Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2008 Jul 1;11(2):203-223.
doi: 10.1017/S1366728908003386.

Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development

Affiliations

Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development

Ioulia Kovelman et al. Biling (Camb Engl). .

Abstract

How does age of first bilingual language exposure affect reading development in children learning to read in both of their languages? Is there a reading advantage for monolingual English children who are educated in bilingual schools? We studied children (grades 2-3, ages 7-9) in bilingual Spanish-English schools who were either from Spanish-speaking homes (new to English) or English-speaking homes (new to Spanish), as compared with English-speaking children in monolingual English schools. An early age of first bilingual language exposure had a positive effect on reading, phonological awareness, and language competence in both languages: early bilinguals (age of first exposure 0-3 years) outperformed other bilingual groups (age of first exposure 3-6 years). Remarkably, schooling in two languages afforded children from monolingual English homes an advantage in phoneme awareness skills. Early bilingual exposure is best for dual language reading development, and it may afford such a powerful positive impact on reading and language development that it may possibly ameliorate the negative effect of low SES on literacy. Further, age of first bilingual exposure provides a new tool for evaluating whether a young bilingual has a reading problem versus whether he or she is a typically-developing dual-language learner.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Language Competence/Expressive Proficiency Task. Sample frames/events from the cartoon.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(a) In English, group (iv) performed the same as group (i) and better than groups (ii–iii) on phoneme awareness, reading and language tasks; (b) in Spanish, groups (i) and (ii) performed the same and better than group (iv) on reading and language tasks; all groups performed equally high on Spanish phoneme awareness (p < 0.01).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Bilingual school students from monolingual English-speaking homes, high SES, significantly outperformed matched monolingual school students, also high SES, on a challenging phoneme awareness task (p < 0.01).

References

    1. Adams MJ. Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1994.
    1. August D, Hakuta K, editors. Improving schooling for language-minority children: A research agenda. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1997.
    1. Berens MS, Kovelman I, Petitto LA. Teaching reading in two languages: Should we teach both languages at the same time or in sequence?. Abstract accepted as a poster presentation at the European Second Language Association Conference; Newcastle, UK. 2007.
    1. Berninger VW, Abbott RD, Billingsley R, Nagy W. Processes underlying timing and fluency of reading: Efficiency, automaticity, coordination, and morphological awareness. In: Wolf M, editor. Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. Timonium, MD: York Press; 2001. pp. 383–414.
    1. Bialystok E. Letters, sounds, and symbols: Changes in children's understanding of written language. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1991;12:75–89.

LinkOut - more resources