Dissociating emotion-induced blindness and hypervision
- PMID: 20001129
- DOI: 10.1037/a0017749
Dissociating emotion-induced blindness and hypervision
Abstract
Previous findings suggest that emotional stimuli sometimes improve (emotion-induced hypervision) and sometimes impair (emotion-induced blindness) the visual perception of subsequent neutral stimuli. We hypothesized that these differential carryover effects might be due to 2 distinct emotional influences in visual processing. On the one hand, emotional stimuli trigger a general enhancement in the efficiency of visual processing that can carry over onto other stimuli. On the other hand, emotional stimuli benefit from a stimulus-specific enhancement in later attentional processing at the expense of competing visual stimuli. We investigated whether detrimental (blindness) and beneficial (hypervision) carryover effects of emotion in perception can be dissociated within a single experimental paradigm. In 2 experiments, we manipulated the temporal competition for attention between an emotional cue word and a subsequent neutral target word by varying cue-target interstimulus interval (ISI) and cue visibility. Interestingly, emotional cues impaired target identification at short ISIs but improved target identification when competition was diminished by either increasing ISI or reducing cue visibility, suggesting that emotional significance of stimuli can improve and impair visual performance through distinct perceptual mechanisms.
Similar articles
-
Auditory emotional cues enhance visual perception.Cognition. 2010 Apr;115(1):202-6. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.004. Epub 2010 Jan 21. Cognition. 2010. PMID: 20096407
-
Attentional capture by emotional stimuli is modulated by semantic processing.J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2008 Apr;34(2):328-39. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.328. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2008. PMID: 18377174
-
The costs of emotional attention: affective processing inhibits subsequent lexico-semantic analysis.J Cogn Neurosci. 2007 Dec;19(12):1932-49. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.1932. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007. PMID: 17892390
-
Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging.Neuropsychologia. 2007 Jan 7;45(1):174-94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003. Epub 2006 Jul 18. Neuropsychologia. 2007. PMID: 16854439 Review.
-
Dynamics of emotional effects on spatial attention in the human visual cortex.Prog Brain Res. 2006;156:67-91. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56004-2. Prog Brain Res. 2006. PMID: 17015075 Review.
Cited by
-
Making something out of nothing: neutral content modulates attention in generalized anxiety disorder.Depress Anxiety. 2011 May;28(5):427-34. doi: 10.1002/da.20806. Epub 2011 Mar 29. Depress Anxiety. 2011. PMID: 21449004 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Threat but not arousal narrows attention: evidence from pupil dilation and saccade control.Front Psychol. 2011 Oct 31;2:281. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00281. eCollection 2011. Front Psychol. 2011. PMID: 22059081 Free PMC article.
-
Emotion strengthens high-priority memory traces but weakens low-priority memory traces.Psychol Sci. 2014 Feb;25(2):387-95. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504784. Epub 2013 Dec 5. Psychol Sci. 2014. PMID: 24311478 Free PMC article.
-
Negative arousal increases the effects of stimulus salience in older adults.Exp Aging Res. 2015;41(3):259-71. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2015.1021644. Exp Aging Res. 2015. PMID: 25978446 Free PMC article.
-
Arousal-Biased Competition in Perception and Memory.Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011 Mar;6(2):114-33. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011. PMID: 21660127 Free PMC article.
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources