Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Mar 8:1318:178-87.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.018. Epub 2009 Dec 23.

Optimal performance in a countermanding saccade task

Affiliations

Optimal performance in a countermanding saccade task

Kongfatt Wong-Lin et al. Brain Res. .

Abstract

Countermanding an action is a fundamental form of cognitive control. In a saccade-countermanding task, subjects are instructed that, if a stop signal appears shortly after a target, they are to maintain fixation rather than to make a saccade to the target. In recent years, recordings in the frontal eye fields and superior colliculus of behaving non-human primates have found correlates of such countermanding behavior in movement and fixation neurons. In this work, we extend a previous neural network model of countermanding to account for the high pre-target activity of fixation neurons. We propose that this activity reflects the functioning of control mechanisms responsible for optimizing performance. We demonstrate, using computer simulations and mathematical analysis, that pre-target fixation neuronal activity supports countermanding behavior that maximizes reward rate as a function of the stop signal delay, fraction of stop signal trials, intertrial interval, duration of timeout, and relative reward value. We propose experiments to test these predictions regarding optimal behavior.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
The model reproduces behavioral data. (A) Model architecture with asymmetrical mutual inhibition between movement (MN) and fixation (FN) neural units. (B) Timecourse of inputs to MN (green) and FN (red); SSD=stop signal delay. Cognitive control is assumed to vary pre-target FN input. Experimental data (C, D) and model simulations (E, F); SSDs in legends; axes for (C, D) are the same as (E, F), respectively. (C, E) Probability of noncancelled trials. (D, F) Cumulative distributions of reaction times for noncancelled and no-stop signal trials. (C, D) Adapted from Boucher et al. (2007), with permission; original data is from Hanes et al. (1998).
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
The model’s neural activity timecourse is similar to experimental data. Time course of trial-averaged activities for movement (A, C) and fixation (B, D) neural neurons for no-stop (thin) and cancelled (bold) trials; (A, B) experimental data; (C, D) model simulations. Solid vertical lines in (A, B) denote onset of stop signal; arrows in (A, C) denote onsets of significant deviation of activities between no-stop and stop signal trials. (C, D) show SSDs of 69 ms (light grey), 117 ms (dark grey), and 169 ms (thick black). All data averaged over 200 trials. (A, B) Adapted from Boucher et al. (2007), with permission; original data is from Hanes et al. (1998).
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Model dynamics in phase space. (A) Orbits averaged over many trials; no-stop signal trials shown dashed, cancelled trials for two SSDs solid. (B–D) Vector fields of the noise-free system (arrows) and nullclines for fixation and movement neurons (red and green respectively); nullclines intersect at stable fixed points (black filled circles). (B) Pre-target epoch: fixation neurons stabilize at high firing rate while movement neurons have low activity. (C) Target epoch: movement neuron activities ramp up toward a new high steady state above movement threshold (dashed blue). (D) In stop-signal trials orbits turn to approach a third steady state with high fixation neuron activity.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Effects of pre-target FN activity and stop-signal delay. High FN activities (red) delay ramping up of MN activities (green) in both noncancelled (A) and cancelled (B) trials; darker colors denote higher FN activity; SSD=169 ms. (C, D) Higher pre-target FN activity increases noncancelled mean reaction time (RT) (C) and probability of cancellation P(cancelled) (D); SSDs for (C, D) shown in legend on (C).
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Optimal countermanding with various SSDs and fractions of stop-signal trials. (A, B) Increase in only SSD (A) or fraction of stop-signal trials (B) shifts optimal FN activity to higher values (filled circles), while overall reward rate decreases. (C) Range of reward rate over a range of SSD values increases with increasing fraction of stop signal trials, allowing reward rate slope to change from negative to positive, and hence reward rate maxima to occur. Bold, dashed, dashed-dot, dotted lines denote SSDs 0, 50, 100, and 150 ms, respectively. (A–C) ITI and timeout are 500 ms. Note that vertical scales differ.
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Increasing ITI, timeout or reward value of cancelled trials can reveal reward rate maxima. Fraction of stop signal trials in (A–D) are fixed at 10% such that a TISI and a Ttimeout of 500 ms does not show reward rate maxima. Increasing ITI (B), timeout (C), or reward value of a cancelled trial relative to a no-stop trial, here by FOUR times (D), can produce reward rate maxima (filled circles). Note vertical scales differ.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bogacz R, Brown E, Moehlis J, Holmes P, Cohen JD. The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced choice tasks. Psychol Rev. 2006;113:700–765. - PubMed
    1. Boucher L, Palmeri TJ, Gordan GD, Schall JD. Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. Psych Rev. 2007;114:376–397. - PubMed
    1. Cutsuridis V, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Perantonis S. A neural model of decision-making by the superior colliculus in an antisaccade task. Neural Netw. 2007;20:690–704. - PubMed
    1. Emeric EE, Brown JW, Boucher L, Carpenter RH, Hanes DP, Harris R, Logan GD, Mashru RN, Par ’e M, Pouget P, Stuphorn V, Taylor TL, Schall JD. Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys. Vis Res. 2007;47:35–49. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Everling S, DeSouza JF. Rule-dependent activity for prosaccades and antisaccades in the primate prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17:1483–1496. - PubMed

Publication types