Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Apr;48(5):1356-66.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.038. Epub 2009 Dec 29.

Accessibility of the nondominant language in picture naming: a counterintuitive effect of dementia on bilingual language production

Affiliations

Accessibility of the nondominant language in picture naming: a counterintuitive effect of dementia on bilingual language production

Tamar H Gollan et al. Neuropsychologia. 2010 Apr.

Abstract

The current study tested the assumption that bilinguals with dementia regress to using primarily the dominant language. Spanish-English bilinguals with probable Alzheimer's disease (AD; n=29), and matched bilingual controls (n=42) named Boston Naming Test pictures in their dominant and nondominant languages. Surprisingly, differences between patients and controls were larger using dominant-language than nondominant-language naming scores, and bilinguals with AD were either more likely than controls (in English-dominant bilinguals), or equally likely (in Spanish-dominant bilinguals), to name some pictures in the nondominant language that they could not produce in their dominant language. These findings suggest that dominant language testing may provide the best assessment of language deficits in bilingual AD, and argue against the common notion that the nondominant language is particularly susceptible to dementia. The greater vulnerability of the dominant language may reflect the increased probability of AD affecting richer semantic representations associated with dominant compared to nondominant language names.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean proportion of Boston Naming Test pictures named correctly in each of the four scoring methods in English-dominant bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease and healthy controls. Error bars show standard errors.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean proportion of Boston Naming Test pictures named correctly in each of the four scoring methods in Spanish-dominant bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease and healthy controls. Error bars show standard errors.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Degree of difference in the proportion of pictures named correctly between bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease and healthy controls in each of the four scoring methods.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abutalebi J, Annoni JM, Zimine I, Pegna AJ, Seghier ML, Lee-Jahnke H, Lazeyras F, Cappa SF, Khateb A. Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:1496–1505. - PubMed
    1. Abutalebi J, Cappa SF, Perani D. Functional neuroimaging of the bilingual brain. In: Kroll JFK, De Groot A, editors. Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 497–515.
    1. Acevedo A, Krueger KR, Navarro E, Ortiz F, Manly JJ, Padilla-Velez MM, Weintraub S, Lopez OL, Mungas D. The Spanish translation and adaptation of the Uniform Data Set of the National Institute on Aging Alzheimer's Disease Centers. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2009;23:102–109. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA. Performance on the Boston Naming Test in English-Spanish bilingual older adults: Some considerations. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2007;13:212–214. - PubMed
    1. Adelman JS, Brown GDA, Quesada JF. Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science. 2006;17:814–823. - PubMed

Publication types