Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 May;40(2):209-220.
doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2009.01.001.

Explicit disassociation of a conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus during extinction training reduces both time to asymptotic extinction and spontaneous recovery of a conditioned taste aversion

Affiliations

Explicit disassociation of a conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus during extinction training reduces both time to asymptotic extinction and spontaneous recovery of a conditioned taste aversion

G Andrew Mickley et al. Learn Motiv. 2009 May.

Abstract

Conditioned taste aversions (CTAs) may be acquired when an animal consumes a novel taste (CS) and then experiences the symptoms of poisoning (US). This aversion may be extinguished by repeated exposure to the CS alone. However, following a latency period in which the CS is not presented, the CTA will spontaneously recover (SR). In the current study we employed an explicitly unpaired extinction procedure (EU-EXT) to determine if it could thwart SR of a CTA. Sprague-Dawley rats acquired a strong CTA after 3 pairings of saccharin (SAC the CS) and Lithium Chloride (LiCl the US). CTA acquisition was followed by extinction (EXT) training consisting of either (a) CS-only exposure (CSO) or, (b) exposure to saccharin and Lithium Chloride on alternate days (i.e., explicitly unpaired: EU). Both extinction procedures resulted in >/= 90% reacceptance of SAC, although the EU extinction procedure (EU-EXT) significantly decreased the time necessary for rats to reach this criterion (compared to CSO controls). Rats were subsequently tested for SR of the CTA upon re-exposure to SAC following a 30-day latency period of water drinking. Rats that acquired a CTA and then underwent the CSO extinction procedure exhibited a significant suppression of SAC drinking during the SR test (as compared to their SAC drinking at the end of extinction). However, animals in the EU-EXT group did not show such suppression in drinking compared to CSO controls. These data suggest that the EU-EXT procedure may be useful in reducing both time to extinction and the spontaneous recovery of fears.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean volume of SAC consumption (± SEM) after either three CS+US pairings (CTA) or three explicitly unpaired CS/US exposures (NO CTA). The CTA group showed a significant decrease in the amount of SAC consumed over the three exposures. The NO CTA group showed a significant increase in SAC consumption over the same three periods. This indicates that the CTA groups had acquired the CTA, whereas the NO CTA group did not. * = significantly different from the CTA animals (α = 0.05).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean days (± SEM) for animals to reach asymptotic extinction. Animals either underwent the CTA+EU-EXT or CTA+CSO-EXT procedure. The CTA+EU-EXT group took significantly fewer days to extinguish the learned fear than the CTA+CSO-EXT group. * = significantly different from the CTA+CSO-EXT animals (α = 0.05).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean days (± SEM) spent in each phase of extinction (Nolan et al., 1997) for animals that underwent either CTA+EU-EXT or CTA+CSO-EXT procedures. Rats that experienced the CTA+EU-EXT procedure spent significantly fewer days in the static phase (SAC reacceptance of less than 10% of baseline) than the CTA+CSO-EXT group [* = significantly different from the CTA+CSO-EXT animals (α = 0.05)]. However, the CTA+EU-EXT and CTA+CSO-EXT groups spent comparable number of days in both the dynamic (SAC reacceptance greater than 10 % but less than 80% from baseline) and asymptotic phases (SAC reacceptance of greater than 80% from baseline).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean volume of SAC consumption (± SEM) on the day of asymptotic extinction and on the subsequent SR test day for both the CTA+EU-EXT and CTA+CSO-EXT animals. On the day of the final extinction test, the CTA+EU-EXT and CTA+CSO-EXT groups drank comparable amounts of SAC (p > 0.05; see text). The CTA+CSO-EXT group drank significantly more SAC on the last day of extinction than on the day of the SR test, indicating a spontaneous recovery of the CTA. However, the CTA+EU-EXT animals drank nearly the same amount of SAC on the day of extinction as they did on the SR test day, suggesting that the EU-EXT procedure may be effective in blocking SR. * = groups indicated are significantly different, α= 0.05.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baeyens F, Eelen P, Crombez G. Pavlovian associations are forever: On classical conditioning and extinction. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1995;9:127–141.
    1. Baker AG. Conditioned inhibition arising from a between-sessions negative correlation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1977;3:144–155.
    1. Barad M. Is extinction of fear erasure or inhibition? Why both, of course. Learning and Memory. 2006;13:108–109. - PubMed
    1. Basoglu M, Salcioglu E, Livanou M. A randomized controlled study of single-session behavioral treatment of earthquake-related post-traumatic stress disorder using and earthquake simulator. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:203–213. - PubMed
    1. Best MB. Conditioned and latent inhibition in taste-aversion learning: Clarifying the role of learned safety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Animal Behavioral Processes. 1975;104:97–113. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources