Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Apr;57(4-5):488-95.
doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.02.007. Epub 2010 Feb 19.

Exogenous androgen during development alters adult partner preference and mating behavior in gonadally intact male rats

Affiliations

Exogenous androgen during development alters adult partner preference and mating behavior in gonadally intact male rats

C L Henley et al. Horm Behav. 2010 Apr.

Abstract

In the rat, neonatal administration of testosterone propionate to a castrated male causes masculinization of behavior. However, if an intact male is treated neonatally with testosterone (hyper-androgen condition), male sexual behavior in adulthood is disrupted. There is a possibility that the hyper-androgen treatment is suppressing male sexual behavior by altering the male's partner preference and thereby reducing his motivation to approach the female. If so, this would suggest that exposure to supra-physiological levels of androgen during development may result in the development of male-oriented partner preference in the male. To test this idea, male rats were treated either postnatally or prenatally with testosterone, and partner preference and sexual behavior were examined in adulthood. The principal finding of this study was that increased levels of testosterone during early postnatal life, but not prenatal, decreased male sexual behavior and increased the amount of time a male spent with a stimulus male, without affecting the amount of time spent with a stimulus female during partner preference tests. Thus, the reduction in male sexual behavior produced by early exposure to high levels of testosterone is not likely due to a reduction in the male's motivation to approach a receptive female.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Partner preference data (Experiment 1). In Experiment 1, the experimental males were exposed to early postnatal treatments and stimulus males were untreated. A) The C males showed a higher preference score than did the TP males in the partner preference tests. Preference score is calculated as time spent with stimulus female minus the time spent with stimulus male. B) The TP males spent more time in the stimulus male chamber than did the C males. C) The amount of time spent with the stimulus female was not affected by early postnatal treatment. TP n=17, C n=12 *Significantly different from C males. #Significantly different from Final test.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Partner preference data (Experiment 2). In Experiment 2, the experimental males were exposed to early postnatal treatments and stimulus males were treated with ATD. A) The C males showed a trend for a higher preference score than did the TP males in the partner preference tests. Preference score is calculated as time spent with stimulus female minus the time spent with stimulus male. B) The TP males spent more time in the stimulus male chamber than did the C males. C) The amount of time spent with the stimulus female was not affected by early postnatal treatment. TP n=17, C n=16 *Significantly different from C males. #Significantly different from Final test.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Partner preference data (Experiment 3). In Experiment 3, the experimental males were exposed to prenatal treatments and stimulus males were untreated. A) All males increased their preference score from initial to final test. Preference score is calculated as time spent with stimulus female minus the time spent with stimulus male. B) All males decreased the amount of time spent with the stimulus male from initial to final test. C) All males increased the amount of time spent with the stimulus female from initial to final test. TP n=7, C n=7 #Significantly different from Final test.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adkins-Regan E. Sex hormones and sexual orientation in animals. Psychobiology. 1988;16(4):335–347.
    1. Bakker J. Sexual differentiation of the neuroendocrine mechanisms regulating mate recognition in mammals. Journal Of Neuroendocrinology. 2003;15(6):615–621. - PubMed
    1. Bakker J, vanOphemert J, Slob AK. Sexual differentiation of odor and partner preference in the rat. Physiology & Behavior. 1996;60(2):489–494. - PubMed
    1. Bakker J, Vanophemert J, Timmerman MA, Dejong FH, Slob AK. Endogenous reproductive hormones and nocturnal rhythms in partner preference and sexual behavior of ATD-treated male rats. Neuroendocrinology. 1995;62(4):396–405. - PubMed
    1. Baum MJ. Differentiation Of Coital Behavior In Mammals - Comparative-Analysis. Neuroscience And Biobehavioral Reviews. 1979;3(4):265–284. - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms