Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Feb 19;5(2):e9296.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009296.

Sugar overconsumption during adolescence selectively alters motivation and reward function in adult rats

Affiliations

Sugar overconsumption during adolescence selectively alters motivation and reward function in adult rats

Leandro F Vendruscolo et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Background: There has been a dramatic escalation in sugar intake in the last few decades, most strikingly observed in the adolescent population. Sugar overconsumption has been associated with several adverse health consequences, including obesity and diabetes. Very little is known, however, about the impact of sugar overconsumption on mental health in general, and on reward-related behavioral disorders in particular. This study examined in rats the effects of unlimited access to sucrose during adolescence on the motivation for natural and pharmacological rewards in adulthood.

Methodology/principal findings: Adolescent rats had free access to 5% sucrose or water from postnatal day 30 to 46. The control group had access to water only. In adulthood, rats were tested for self-administration of saccharin (sweet), maltodextrin (non-sweet), and cocaine (a potent drug of abuse) using fixed- and progressive-ratio schedules, and a concentration-response curve for each substance. Adult rats, exposed or not exposed to sucrose, were tested for saccharin self-administration later in life to verify the specificity of adolescence for the sugar effects. Sugar overconsumption during adolescence, but not during adulthood, reduced the subsequent motivation for saccharin and maltodextrin, but not cocaine. This selective decrease in motivation is more likely due to changes in brain reward processing than changes in gustatory perception.

Conclusions/significance: Sugar overconsumption induces a developmental stage-specific chronic depression in reward processing that may contribute to an increase in the vulnerability to reward-related psychiatric disorders.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Motivation for saccharin (sweet) in adult rats exposed to sucrose or water during adolescence.
(A) Percentage of rats reaching the acquisition criterion (>50 reinforcements for 2 consecutive sessions) for saccharin (0.13%, w/v) self-administration; (B) Number of saccharin (0.13%, w/v) reinforcements earned on fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) and fixed-ratio 5 (FR5) schedules; (C) Breakpoint reached during the progressive-ratio (PR) test; and (D) Number of reinforcements earned (FR5) at different saccharin concentrations (in %, w/v), by adult rats exposed to sucrose (N = 15) or water (N = 16) during adolescence. *, **, *** Indicate significant group differences (p<0.05, p<0.01 and p<0.001, respectively).
Figure 2
Figure 2. Motivation for maltodextrin (non-sweet) in adult rats exposed to sucrose or water during adolescence.
(A) Percentage of rats reaching the acquisition criterion (>50 reinforcements for 2 consecutive sessions) for maltodextrin (5%, w/v) self-administration; (B) Number of maltodextrin (5%, w/v) reinforcements earned on fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) and fixed-ratio 5 (FR5) schedules; (C) Breakpoint reached during the progressive-ratio (PR) test; and (D) Number of reinforcements earned (FR5) at different maltodextrin concentrations (in %, w/v), by adult rats exposed to sucrose (N = 8) or water (N = 8) during adolescence. * Indicates significant group differences (p<0.05).
Figure 3
Figure 3. Motivation for cocaine in adult rats exposed to sucrose or water during adolescence.
(A) Percentage of rats reaching the acquisition criterion (>15 reinforcements for 2 consecutive sessions) for cocaine (250 µg/infusion) self-administration; (B) Number of cocaine (250 µg/infusion) reinforcements earned on fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) and fixed-ratio 5 (FR5) schedules; (C) Breakpoint reached during the progressive-ratio (PR) test; and (D) Number of reinforcements earned (FR5) at different cocaine concentrations (in µg/infusion), by adult rats exposed to sucrose (N = 10) or water (N = 9) during adolescence.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Motivation for saccharin (sweet) in rats exposed to sucrose or water during adulthood.
(A) Percentage of rats reaching the acquisition criterion (>50 reinforcements for 2 consecutive sessions) for saccharin (0.13%, w/v) self-administration; (B) Number of saccharin (0.13%, w/v) reinforcements earned on fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) and fixed-ratio 5 (FR5) schedules; (C) Breakpoint reached during the progressive-ratio (PR) test; and (D) Number of reinforcements earned (FR5) at different saccharin concentrations (in %, w/v), by rats exposed to sucrose (N = 8) or water (N = 8) during adulthood. * Indicates significant group differences (p<0.05).
Figure 5
Figure 5. Motivation for saccharin (sweet) in adult rats exposed to saccharin or water during adolescence.
(A) Percentage of rats reaching the acquisition criterion (>50 reinforcements for 2 consecutive sessions) for saccharin (0.13%, w/v) self-administration; (B) Number of saccharin (0.13%, w/v) reinforcements earned on fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) and fixed-ratio 5 (FR5) schedules; (C) Break point reached during the progressive-ratio (PR) test; and (D) Number of reinforcements earned (FR5) at different saccharin concentrations (in %, w/v), by adult rats exposed to saccharin (N = 8) or water (N = 8) during adolescence. *, ** Indicate significant group differences (p<0.05 and p<0.01, respectively).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Mintz SW. Time, sugar and sweetness. Marx Persp. 1979;2(4):56–73. - PubMed
    1. Jacobson MF. Washington, DC: Center for Science in the Public Interest; 2005. Liquid Candy: How Soft Drinks Are Harming Americans' Health. 2nd ed.
    1. World Health Organization. Geneva: WHO; 2003. Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Joint WHO/FAO Expert consultation. WHO Technical Report Series 916. - PubMed
    1. Popkin BM, Nielsen SJ. The sweetening of the world's diet. Obes Res. 2003;11:1325–1332. - PubMed
    1. Guthrie JF, Morton JF. Food sources of added sweeteners in the diets of Americans. J Am Diet Assoc. 2000;100 (1):43–48, 51. - PubMed

Publication types

MeSH terms