Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Fall;42(3):511-25.
doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-511.

Do children prefer contingencies? An evaluation of the efficacy of and preference for contingent versus noncontingent social reinforcement during play

Affiliations

Do children prefer contingencies? An evaluation of the efficacy of and preference for contingent versus noncontingent social reinforcement during play

Kevin C Luczynski et al. J Appl Behav Anal. 2009 Fall.

Abstract

Discovering whether children prefer reinforcement via a contingency or independent of their behavior is important considering the ubiquity of these programmed schedules of reinforcement. The current study evaluated the efficacy of and preference for social interaction within differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA) and noncontingent reinforcement (NCR) schedules with typically developing children. Results showed that 7 of the 8 children preferred the DRA schedule; 1 child was indifferent. We also demonstrated a high degree of procedural fidelity, which suggested that preference is influenced by the presence of a contingency under which reinforcement can be obtained. These findings are discussed in terms of (a) the selection of reinforcement schedules in practice, (b) variables that influence children's preferences for contexts, and (c) the selection of experimental control procedures when evaluating the effects of reinforcement.

Keywords: concurrent-chains arrangement; contingency strength; differential reinforcement; noncontingent reinforcement; preference assessment.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Responses per minute for “Kevin” (first row) and “excuse me” (second row) during the contingent reinforcement schedule (CR; open triangles) and no-reinforcement schedule (no Sr+; filled circles) and cumulative initial-link selections during the preference assessment (third row) for Amy, Cia, Dee, and Ted across sessions. The data depicted prior to the phase line were collected during the efficacy evaluation, and the data after were collected during the preference assessment.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Responses per minute for “Kevin” (first row) and “excuse me” (second row) during the CR schedule (open triangles) and no-Sr+ schedule (filled circles) and cumulative initial-link selections during the preference assessment (third row) for Beth, Ed, Sam, and Lou across sessions. The data depicted prior to the phase line were collected during the efficacy evaluation, and the data after were collected during the preference assessment.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Responses per minute for “excuse me” (first row) and “Kevin” (second row) during differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA; open circles), noncontingent reinforcement (NCR; filled triangles), and extinction (EXT; filled circles), and cumulative initial-link selections during the preference assessment (third row) for Amy, Cia, Dee, and Ted across sessions. The data depicted prior to the phase line were collected during the efficacy evaluation, and the data after were collected during the preference assessment.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Responses per minute for “excuse me” (first row) and “Kevin” (second row) during DRA (open circles), NCR (filled triangles), and EXT (filled circles) across sessions, and cumulative initial-link selections during the preference assessment (third row) for Beth, Ed, Sam, and Lou across sessions. The data depicted prior to the phase line were collected during the efficacy evaluation, and the data after were collected during the preference assessment.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Mean contingency strengths for “excuse me” (dotted line) and “Kevin” (solid line) in the DRA (first and third rows) and NCR schedules (second and fourth rows) across progressively increasing window sizes for all children. All sessions from both the efficacy evaluation and preference assessment for each schedule were included in these analyses.
Figure 6
Figure 6
The number of response–reinforcer occurrences experienced in the last three sessions of NCR during the efficacy evaluation for “excuse me” (filled triangles) and “Kevin” (gray triangles) across progressively increasing window sizes for all children.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Durand V.M. Functional communication training using assistive devices: Recruiting natural communities of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1999;32:247–267. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Fisher W, Piazza C.C, Bowman L.G, Hagopian L.P, Owens J.C, Slevin I. A comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1992;25:491–498. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hagopian L.P, Fisher W.W, Legacy S.M. Schedule effects of noncontingent reinforcement on attention-maintained destructive behavior in identical quadruplets. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1994;27:317–325. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hall R.V, Lund D, Jackson D. Effects of teacher attention on study behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1968;1:1–12. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hammond L.J. The effect of contingency upon the appetitive conditioning of free-operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1980;34:297–304. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources