Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2009 Fall;42(3):575-93.
doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-575.

Equivalence class formation: a method for teaching statistical interactions

Affiliations

Equivalence class formation: a method for teaching statistical interactions

Lanny Fields et al. J Appl Behav Anal. 2009 Fall.

Abstract

Many students struggle with statistical concepts such as interaction. In an experimental group, participants took a paper-and-pencil test and then were given training to establish equivalent classes containing four different statistical interactions. All participants formed the equivalence classes and showed maintenance when probes contained novel negative exemplars. Thereafter, participants took a second paper-and-pencil test. Participants in the control group received two versions of the paper-and-pencil test without equivalence-based instruction. All participants in the experimental group showed increased paper-and-pencil test scores after forming the interaction-indicative equivalence classes. Class-indicative responding also generalized to novel exemplars and the novel question format used in the paper-and-pencil test. Test scores did not change with repetition for control group participants. Implications for behavioral diagnostics and teaching technology are discussed.

Keywords: college students; computer-based training; equivalence classes; generalization to novel exemplars.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
An example of the four members of each class of stimuli used during the equivalence training.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Two examples of questions on the paper pretest and posttest. The first question tests a B-A relation from Class 2, and the second question tests a D-C relation from Class 4.
Figure 3
Figure 3
The mean number of blocks needed for all experimental group participants to achieve mastery criterion during the computer-based equivalence training. Each training and testing phase of equivalence class formation appears as a separate bar, and the left to right position of each bar corresponds to the order in which each relation was trained or tested. The height of each bar indicates the mean number of blocks needed to form a baseline relation or to pass an emergent relations test.
Figure 4
Figure 4
The mean pretest–posttest scores for both experimental (filled squares) and control (open circles) groups. The I beams that bracket each data point indicated ± 1 SE.
Figure 5
Figure 5
A scattergram showing posttest scores plotted as function of pretest scores for each participant in the experimental (filled circles) and control (open circles) groups. Two participants in the experimental group produced identical pretest and posttest scores, indicated by the arrow. Separate regression lines are also shown for the data obtained from participants in the experimental and control groups.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adams B.J, Fields L, Verhave T. Formation of generalized equivalence classes. The Psychological Record. 1993;43:553–566.
    1. Arntzen E, Holth P. Probability of stimulus equivalence as a function of training design. The Psychological Record. 1997;47:309–320.
    1. Belanich J, Fields L. Generalized equivalence classes as response transfer networks. The Psychological Record. 2003;53:373–414.
    1. Bradstreet T.E. Teaching introductory statistics courses so that nonstatisticians experience statistical reasoning. The American Statistician. 1996;50:69–78.
    1. Branch M. Stimulus generalization, stimulus equivalence, and response hierarchies. In: Hayes S.C, Hayes L.J, Sato M, Ono K, editors. Behavior analysis of language and cognition. Reno, NV: Context Press; 1994. pp. 51–70. In.

LinkOut - more resources